Do photographs convey objective information to a viewer? In common intuition, they do, being the result of an automatic, mechanical process (
Costello, 2017), and so photography holds a special place in how we convey information, whether in journalism, legal proceedings, or social media, and many other areas. Indeed, current fears around the dangers of image manipulation imply a belief in the honesty of unmanipulated images. More nuanced discussions highlight choices made by photographers: a photographer chooses the subject and aims the camera, and selects zoom and exposure settings. These choices determine the content and the aesthetic qualities of an image (
Palmer, Schloss, & Sammartino, 2013); much has been written about these subjective choices in documentary photography (
Bersak, 2006;
Morris, 2014) and social media (
Hawley, 2022). But, one might think that, once the photographer presses the shutter-release button, the rest of the imaging process is an objective measurement and display of light. Indeed, in perception and art history, many texts define an ideal
picture as one that displays light as though the viewer were looking through a window into the depicted scene (
Gibson, 1971;
Kemp, 1990;
Pirenne, 1970;
Yang & Kubovy, 1999). Many of these works describe linear perspective as “correct perspective.” From this, one might conclude that pinhole cameras create correct pictures, which typical consumer cameras approximate with lenses.
This article describes how, rather than being objective measurements of light, all photographs display light according to hidden, subjective choices made by the photographer and camera manufacturer. These choices determine the depictions of tones, colors, and perspective. It is self-evident that a representational painter must make all of these choices, but, in photography, many of these choices are hidden in optical, mechanical, chemical, and/or software design choices. These choices embed both perceptual and aesthetic preferences, just as they do in painting. Such choices are mandatory. There is no single correct way to make a picture; conventional photographs almost never display light as though the viewer were looking through a window.
Hence, rather than being objective display of light measurements, photography is one type of visual depiction—it is a class of techniques for arranging colors and tones on a flat surface to convey information (
Durand, 2007;
Gibson, 1971). Hence, studies on pictorial space should begin from this assumption, rather than the assumption that linear perspective and linear tones are correct, but that artists sometimes deviate from them. Further research is needed to understand the nature of depiction of pictorial space with these choices. The computational photography techniques discussed in this article could enable systematic new research on depiction choices in art and photography that could, in turn, lead to new depiction techniques. Another implication is that, when used as experimental stimuli, the differences between photographs and real-world perception may affect the results, even when they are carefully calibrated for accuracy (
Snow & Culham, 2021).
The reader is encouraged to try the following informal experiment: take a picture with a smartphone and compare its depictions against real-world appearances, including brightnesses, colors, and relative sizes of objects. On its own, the photograph may look very real, an accurate depiction of the scene. But, on close inspection, one may notice significant differences between the photo and the world—especially in large-scale scenes with significant lighting variations, like a sunlit mountain range or a nighttime city street. This difference is surprising for some viewers (
Albert & Efros, 2016), and can help one to appreciate the subjective choices made, even in seemingly automatic, realistic smartphone photography, and how such choices can seem to be correct if not inspected closely.