However, single unit studies in macaque disagree as to the presence and amount of attentional modulation in V1. Some report statistically significant effects (Chen et al.,
2008; Herrero et al.,
2008; Ito & Gilbert,
1999; McAdams & Reid,
2005; Motter,
1993; Roelfsema, Lamme, & Spekreijse,
1998; Thiele, Pooresmaeili, Delicato, Herrero, & Roelfsema,
2009), while others find little evidence of early attentional gain control (Luck, Chelazzi, Hillyard, & Desimone,
1997; Marcus & Essen,
2002; McAdams & Maunsell,
1999; Moran & Desimone,
1985). Human neuroimaging experiments are less equivocal and invariably find robust attentionally driven changes in the primary visual cortex (Buracas & Boynton,
2007; Herrmann et al.,
2010; Li, Lu, Tjan, Dosher, & Chu,
2008; Tootell et al.,
1998) and even in subcortical structures (O'Connor, Fukui, Pinsk, & Kastner,
2002; Schneider & Kastner,
2009). Finally, human electrophysiological studies often report (Clark & Hillyard,
1996; Luck & Hillyard,
1994; Mangun, Buonocore, Girelli, & Jha,
1998; Martínez et al.,
1999; Russo, Martínez, & Hillyard,
2003; Wang, Clementz, & Keil,
2007) attentional modulation in the visual cortex, but the spatial origins of these signals can usually only be estimated by indirect means (for example, by inspection of signal timing or the dependence of waveforms on stimulus location). Direct estimates of neural modulation from electrophysiological measurements with higher spatial precision differ considerably. For example, our group has recently demonstrated robust attentional modulation in V1 using a source imaging method similar to the one described in this paper (Lauritzen, Ales, & Wade,
2010).