We are also not aware of prior studies reporting an attentional oblique effect. Such an effect is not implausible, however, given the considerable prior evidence that visual attention is not homogenous across the visual field. For example, it has been argued that attention influences crowding (Chakravarthi & Cavanagh,
2007,
2009a,
2009b; but see Freeman & Pelli,
2007)—and the critical region for crowding varies across the visual field. Indeed, crowding's critical region extends further along the radial than tangential axis to fixation (Feng, Jiang, & He,
2007; Toet & Levi,
1992) and is not homogeneous within and across lateral hemifields (Chakravarthi & Cavanagh,
2009a; Liu, Jiang, Sun, & He,
2009). Additionally, attentional performance is often better in the lower than upper visual field (Carrasco, Giordano, & McElree,
2004; He, Cavanagh, & Intrilligator,
1996; McAnany & Levine,
2007; Montaser-Kouhsari & Carrasco,
2009; Rubin, Nakayama, & Shapley,
1996). He et al. (
1996) suggested that this lower field advantage could arise in the parietal lobe, an attention-related region (Buschman & Miller,
2007) that receives more projections corresponding to the lower than the upper visual field (Van Essen, Newsome, & Maunsell,
1984). A conceptually similar argument—an overrepresentation of cardinally tuned neurons in the primary visual cortex (Li et al.,
2003; Mansfield,
1974)—is traditionally offered to explain the oblique effect in stimulus-driven vision.