Previous studies have shown that we are readily able to extrapolate an object's motion path. For example, Ramachandran and Anstis (
1983) used apparent motion displays in which the perceived direction of motion of dots was initially ambiguous. By adding neighboring dots, the authors manipulated the perceived direction of apparent motion and found that motion extrapolation can indeed bias the perceived direction of motion. The authors postulated that “if an object has once been seen moving in one direction, there is a strong tendency to continue seeing motion in that direction” (Ramachandran & Anstis,
1983, p.84). This suggests that an object's trajectory is extrapolated using the Gestalt law of good continuation (Ramachandran & Anstis,
1983; cf. Wertheimer,
1950): An object that is moving along a straight line is expected to continue to move in a similar direction. This can also be seen in a more recent study by Lyon and Waag (
1995) who found that participants can accurately extrapolate a target's position in case of constant velocity circular motion paths. More related to the current study, with an emphasis on the role of attention, when a moving object suddenly disappears and an observer is asked to localize its final position, the observer's estimation is typically shifted in the direction of motion (e.g., Gray & Thornton,
2001; Hubbard,
1995; Iordanescu, Grabowecky, & Suzuki,
2009; Kerzel,
2003a,
2003b). However, when attention is diverted between the moment of object disappearance and response, the forward shift disappears (Kerzel,
2003a). This suggests that directed attention is a prerequisite for motion extrapolation. Verghese and McKee (
2002) showed that motion extrapolation may occur also in a more automatic manner. They found that when, within a time window of 70–100 ms, unidirectional movement of a single dot is detected (among randomly moving dots), attention is automatically allocated to the subsequent segments of its trajectory leading to enhanced detectability of the dot. Recently Howard, Masom, and Holcombe (
2011) reported that, in a multiple object tracking task (MOT, see below), after the disappearance of a target the remembered target location lagged behind the actual target location. These findings seem to be at odds with the findings that observers anticipate in the direction of motion. Howard et al. (
2011) suggested that a combination of factors may have caused the lag without providing a definite conclusion. For example, they argued that encoding in short term memory might have caused the lag they found in their particular task as opposed to anticipatory processes as found in other tasks (as in, e.g., Iordanescu et al.,
2009). In the current study, instead of focusing on remembered object locations, we focus on the attentional spread around targets during the actual movement.