The history of visual arts has seen numerous attempts to represent aspects from our external world which go beyond the inherently two-dimensional nature of a painting (Gombrich,
1977). For example, painters have developed a pictorial language to invoke clear impressions of depth and motion, which have no direct way of being captured on the flat and static surface of a canvas. In the twentieth century, Op artists began to experiment with simple black and white patterns that can create vivid dynamic illusions in static pictures (Riley,
1995; Wade,
2003), which allow artists to
elicit particular percepts rather than just to
represent aspects of a scene such as motion. Op Art paintings such as Bridget Riley's ‘Fall’ (1963, Tate Britain, London) create vivid sensations of shimmering and movement, despite the fact that they are nothing more than simple static patterns of paint on a static canvas. Similar phenomena related to compositions of repetitive lines in different orientations can be observed in R. Neal's painting ‘Square of three’ (Kupin, Haddad, & Steinman,
1973). The physiological and perceptual mechanisms responsible for motion illusions in such patterns have been a matter of debate for several decades (see, for instance, Mon-Williams & Wann,
1996; Wade,
2003), reaching back to some early descriptions of dynamic deformations observed in patterns that are composed of fine lines (Purkinje, 1828 as discussed in von Helmholtz,
1924). An example of such a continuing debate concerns the mechanisms underlying the percepts in the ‘Enigma’ painting (Leviant,
1982,
1996), which consists of a pattern of radiating black and white lines (similar to the radial line pattern used by MacKay,
1957) superimposed by concentric rings of uniform color. This painting elicits two types of motion illusions, namely, circular motion in the concentric rings and a shimmering motion in the radiating lines. Scientists have speculated whether higher cortical mechanisms are required to explain such phenomena (see, for instance, MacKay,
1957; Zeki,
1994; Zeki & Lamb,
1994; Zeki, Watson, & Frackowiak,
1993), or whether the instability of the eyes, which are continuously moving and at the same time changing focus, could be responsible for the perceived motion (for instance, Pirenne, Campbell, Robson, & Mackay,
1958; Gregory,
1993,
1994; Kupin et al.,
1973). The latter idea has gained some empirical support from studies that have directly compared the frequency of small eye movements with the strength of the motion illusion perceived in the rings of ‘Enigma’ (Troncoso, Macknik, Otero-Millan, & Martinez-Conde,
2008). Although different explanations are often stated as mutually exclusive alternatives, it is possible that the observed phenomena arise from a combination of both perceptual and oculomotor mechanisms.