As we said earlier, S3D displays are believed to cause discomfort and eventually fatigue beyond that experienced with non-stereo displays. Most researchers and engineers have assumed that the symptoms are caused by differences between the stimuli to vergence and accommodation because such differences require the viewer to uncouple vergence and accommodation (Emoto et al.,
2005; Häkkinen et al.,
2006; Howarth & Costello,
1997; Lambooij et al.,
2009; Menozzi,
2000; Ukai,
2007; Wann & Mon-Williams,
2002; Yano et al.,
2004). The evidence offered in support of this hypothesis is that viewers report more discomfort and fatigue when viewing stereo displays than when viewing conventional non-stereo displays (Emoto et al.,
2005; Häkkinen et al.,
2006; Jin, Zhang, Wang, & Plocher,
2007; Yamazaki, Kamijo, & Fukuzumi,
1990; Yano et al.,
2002). This observation, however, does not prove that vergence–accommodation conflicts cause the symptoms because there are several other important differences between viewing non-stereo and stereo displays; these include the eyewear required with stereo displays to separate the two eyes' images, ghosting or crosstalk from one eye's image to the other's image, misalignment of the images presented to the two eyes (Kooi & Toet,
2004), and the perceptual distortions that occur with stereo displays (Bereby-Meyer, Leiser, & Meyer,
1999) and not with non-stereo displays (Vishwanath, Girshick, & Banks,
2005). To show that the vergence–accommodation conflict
per se causes visual discomfort and eventually fatigue, it is essential to have subjects view the same content in the same way with and without conflict. To do this, subjects should view the same stereo imagery when a conflict is present and when it is not present, and the experimenter should then compare the resulting symptoms. To our knowledge, only Hoffman et al. (
2008) have done this, and consequently, that is the only study to have shown convincingly that vergence–accommodation conflict
per se causes discomfort and fatigue. However, Hoffman et al. presented only one base viewing distance (39 cm or 2.5 D) and randomized the sign of the vergence–accommodation conflict. Therefore, they could not determine what the effects of viewing distance and conflict sign are. In the current paper, we present different viewing distances in different sessions in order to determine which distances cause the greatest symptoms. We also present both signs of conflict to determine whether one conflict direction is more uncomfortable than the other.