With monocular occlusions, like the situation in
Figure 10, disparity cannot be measured because one eye cannot see the object. As a consequence, the object's distance cannot be estimated from disparity (Anderson & Nakayama,
1994; Belhumeur & Mumford,
1992). Here we consider the possibility that one can estimate the distance from blur when monocular occlusion occurs.
We assume that the observer is converged and focused on the edge of the occluder at distance
d 0. A small object at distance
d 1 is visible to the left eye only. The object is myopically focused creating a blurred image at the retina. (
Figure 14) The relationship between the dioptric difference in distance and blur magnitude is the same as the hyperopic situation in
Figure 1 (except that the sign of defocus is opposite). From
Equation 3, the object at
d 1 creates a blur circle of diameter
β 1 in radians. We can then solve for
d 1:
The observer can estimate
d 0 from vergence and vertical disparity (Backus, Banks, van Ee, & Crowell,
1999; Gårding, Porrill, Mayhew, & Frisby,
1995). The pupil diameter
A is presumably unknown to the observer, but steady-state diameter does not vary significantly. For luminances of 5–2500 cd/m
2, a range that encompasses typical indoor and outdoor scenes, steady-state diameter varies by only ±1.5 mm within an individual (de Groot & Gebhard,
1952). Thus it is reasonable to assume that the mean of
A is 3 mm with a standard deviation of 0.6 mm. The blur-circle diameter
b1 also has to be estimated, which requires an assumption about the spatial-frequency distribution of the stimulus (Pentland,
1987). The estimation of blur-circle diameter will also be subject to error (Mather & Smith,
2002). We can then treat the estimates of
d0,
A, and
β1 as probability distributions and find the distribution of
d1. In principle then, one can estimate the distance of a partially occluded object from its blur. Can this be done in practice?
Figure 15 demonstrates that blur can indeed affect perceived distance in the monocular occlusion situation. The three stereograms in panel A depict a textured surface that is partially occluding a more distant small object. The blur of the small object increases from the top to bottom row. Most viewers perceive the object as more distant in the middle row than in the top row and more distant in the bottom than in the middle row. Thus blur can aid the estimation of distance in a situation where disparity cannot provide an estimate.
Panel B depicts the same 3D layout but the small object is farther to the left such that it is visible to both eyes. In this case, the effect of blur on perceived distance is reduced, presumably because disparity now specifies object distance.