Overshoot artifacts recorded at the end of saccades by the eye tracker were recognized as an undesirable artifact of eye movements and have been inferred to be lens wobble artifacts accompanying saccades (Crane & Steele,
1978; Deubel & Bridgeman,
1995; Schachar, Davila, Pierscionek, Chen, & Ward,
2007). Since the lens wobble artifacts were not consistent from saccade to saccade, Crane and Steele (
1978) regarded it as unlikely that the artifacts were systematic errors from the instrumentation servomotors. They considered that the artifact reflected crystalline lens wobble. A recent study (Schultz et al.,
2009) has also independently recorded lens wobble artifacts using a 1000-Hz digital video camera to capture P1 and P4. They found similar amplitudes of saccade overshoots in the P4 motion, confirming that the overshoots recorded by dual Purkinje trackers are due to lens wobble. Bahill et al. (
1975a,
1975b) described overshoots that also occurred at the end of saccade. They called it “dynamic overshoot” and their existence was later verified by Kapoula et al. (
1986). These studies concluded that dynamic overshoot resulted from a braking pulse at the end of a saccade and served no useful purpose. Therefore, this kind of overshoot has a saccadic or neural origin. Similarly, it is necessary to justify that the overshoots recorded by dPi eye tracker have a lens origin before using it as a tool measuring lens wobble artifact. Deubel and Bridgeman (
1995) simultaneously used both a dPi eye tracker and magnetic search coils (Collewijn,
1977; Robinson,
1963) to measure saccades. Theoretically, subtracting the recordings from these two methods can provide pure lens motion profiles. However, one potential problem with this approach could come from different mechanic characteristics between the eye tracker and a search coil. If there is latency between these two recordings, this will produce errors. In addition, coils could slip during saccades since cyclotorsion might occur. Methodology in the current study could avoid this kind of error. Commonly, two analog channels (i.e.,
θH/
θV channels) are used in the dPi eye tracker. These two channels include both P1 and P4 signals and are considered to represent pure eye movement. Since the isolated P4 signal is necessary to get the lens wobble effect, two other analog channels record P1 (i.e., H
1/V
1 channels). An amplitude correlation was applied to calibrate the amplitudes between the channels. The subtracted profiles therefore represent lens motion without a saccadic component. Almost all the subtracted profiles show the lens wobble artifact. Some of the artifacts were as large as the saccades. The lens wobble artifact occurrence and amplitudes recorded in the current study are much larger than 13% and 0.15° reported by Kapoula et al. (
1986). Since the approach used in the current study, namely subtracting H
1/V
1 from
θH/
θV can still include head movement artifact, further frequency analysis was performed by comparing the profiles immediately before saccades and during saccades. The results showed that in the older subjects who had some head tremors, the frequency of head movements was less than 5 Hz in general compared with a lens wobble frequency of around 20 Hz. Furthermore, the recordings from the eye tracker were fairly consistent with extracted traces from video clips in which the Purkinje image movements were compared directly with lens wobble as identified from the cuneiform cataract. Therefore, all these different analyses support that the artifacts arise from crystalline lens wobble.