We observed a strong 3D motion aftereffect resulting from prolonged adaptation to unidirectional motion toward or away from the observer (
Figure 5).
Figure 5A depicts the two adaptation conditions: binocularly presented 3D motion toward or away from the observer, followed by binocularly presented test stimuli moving in either the same or opposite direction as adaptation. Data plotted in
Figure 5B show the proportion of “toward” responses as a function of test stimulus coherence (averaged across 3 experienced observers, 72 trials per point, 720 trials total). Increasingly away motion coherence corresponds to negative values on the
x-axis and increasingly toward motion coherence to positive values. A value of 0.5 on the
y-axis corresponds to the point at which an observer is equally likely to report either toward or away on a given trial and indicates an observer's point of subjective equality. The black line shows a logistic fit to subjects' responses prior to adaptation, essentially the observer's 3D direction discrimination sensitivity. As expected, this line is centered on zero motion coherence and has a moderate sensitivity (
α −1 = 0.153, CI
95 = [0.109, 0.197]). Following adaptation to toward-direction motion (green), observers were much more likely to report noisy 3D motion stimuli as moving away (
β = 0.356, CI
95 = [0.307, 0.403]). Similarly, after adaptation to away-direction motion (red), observers were more like to report directionally ambiguous 3D motion as moving toward (
β = −0.557, CI
95 = [−0.508, −0.601]).
Across a range of 3D motion coherences, these 3D MAEs shifted the psychometric functions leftward or rightward (relative to an unadapted control condition) and, thus, could be quantified in terms of relative displacement along the x-axis, i.e., in units of the test stimulus motion coherence. Three-dimensional MAEs were equivalent to about ∼45% (CI95 = [42.4, 48.9]) motion coherence. This effect struck us as surprisingly large: for the test stimuli to be judged as having no net motion on average, approximately half of the dots had to move in the direction opposite that of adaptation.
As a basis for comparison, we measured conventional 2D frontoparallel MAEs for the same observers under stereoscopic viewing conditions (
Figure 6A; dots moved in the same direction in both eyes but were otherwise identical to those used to generate 3D MAEs). We performed the same analysis, except that the directions of motion were leftward or rightward, instead of toward or away. The
y-axis now represents the proportion of rightward responses; negative values on the
x-axis correspond to increasingly leftward motion coherence, and positive values correspond to increasingly rightward motion coherence (
Figure 6B). Unadapted direction discrimination sensitivity (black) is again centered on zero motion coherence but with a noticeable ∼2.5-fold improvement in sensitivity (
α −1 = 0.054, CI
95 = [0.016, 0.076]) over the 3D motion case. Increased direction discrimination sensitivity for 2D motion relative to 3D motion is unsurprising given the previously mentioned stereomotion suppression effect (Tyler,
1971). Further, Welchman, Lam, and Bülthoff (
2008) have shown that greater 2D sensitivity (as measured by increment thresholds) is a consequence of a Bayesian model in which the visual system incorporates a low-speed prior; their model thus predicts this aspect of our data. What is more interesting, however, is the relative magnitude of the 2D and 3D MAEs. The shift in psychometric function following 2D motion adaptation was equivalent to approximately 18% motion coherence (
β = 0.183, CI
95 = [0.165, 0.205]). Given that previous studies on 2D MAEs using similar dynamic test stimuli have observed effects of similar magnitudes (Blake & Hiris,
1993; van Wezel & Britten,
2002), this confirms our initial impression that 3D MAEs are uniquely large.