One of the most important tasks for the visual system is the localization of objects in the world. Anatomical and physiological evidence suggests that there is a functional specialization in visual cortex for processing attributes such as position, form, and motion (Zeki,
1974,
1993). This is complemented by psychophysical evidence from clinical patients showing selective deficits in the perception of visual attributes such as motion after localized damage to cortex (Zihl, von Cramon, & Mai,
1983). However, visual motion can have a pronounced effect on properties of an object such as its position (Whitney,
2002) and size (Whitaker, McGraw, & Pearson,
1999). This suggests that there are significant cortical interactions between neuronal populations coding for these object properties.
Ramachandran and Anstis (
1990) showed that the boundary of a window of coherently moving random dots appears shifted in the direction of motion. De Valois and De Valois (
1991) showed that the perceived position of the static envelope of a drifting Gabor is shifted in a direction normal to the drifting sine wave carrier. These effects demonstrate that the physical presence of motion can shift the perceived position of a window or aperture enclosing the motion. It is also well known that adaptation to motion in one direction can result in a motion after-effect (MAE) in which a subsequently seen static pattern is perceived to move in the opposite direction (for a history of this phenomena, see Wade,
1994). Here motion is perceived although not physically present. A number of studies have investigated whether adaptation to motion can also produce positional after-effects.
Nishida and Johnston (
1999) adapted observers to the rotation of a polar angle grating (windmill pattern grating) and subsequently presented a similar but static test grating. They were able to measure both a standard motion after-effect, in which the static pattern appeared to rotate in a direction opposite to that of the adaptor, and a spatial shift in the perceived position of the pattern, again in a direction opposite to that of the adapting motion. The after-effects had different rates of decay, the spatial shift decaying more slowly than the motion after-effect. This demonstrates that perceived motion
per se can alter the perceived position of stationary pattern and suggests that position and motion are coded separately but interact in determining perceived position (McGraw, Whitaker, Skillen, & Chung,
2002; Snowden,
1998; Whitaker et al.,
1999). The spatial shift produced after adaptation to motion occurs even when the observer is unable to report the direction of motion of the adaptor (Harp, Bressler, & Whitney,
2007; Whitney,
2005).
The perceived position of a static object is thus intimately linked to the present and past motions at the same spatial location. More surprisingly, motion signals from spatially distinct objects can induce remote shifts in the spatial position of static objects, a phenomena we refer to here as “motion drag”. Whitney and Cavanagh (
2000) presented moving gratings to observers and flashed stationary bars either side of the gratings. Although separate from the moving gratings, the bars appeared shifted in the direction of grating motion. This positional shift could be induced even when the moving grating and test objects were separated by up to 60 degrees of visual angle. However, in this experiment distance from the inducing motion was confounded with an increase in visual eccentricity. Durant and Johnston (
2004) found motion drag diminished with distance from the inducing motion in a display in which the eccentricity of the target remained constant, suggesting some local influence. They also examined the time course of motion drag using a rotating bar stimulus. They found that motion drag was maximal when flashed stationary targets were presented 60 ms before the end of the rotating bar came closest to the target position.
Neurons in striate cortex are retinotopically organized, this underlies the traditionally held view that position is coded by the location of activity in a cortical map. This can be traced back to ideas such as Lotze's (
1884) theory of local signs. Effects of motion on perceived position pose a problem for intuitive explanations such as this. It seems unlikely that striate neurons with receptive fields centered on different regions of the visual field would be activated by different directions of motion from a fixed region of visual space (De Valois & De Valois,
1991). Given that primary visual cortex (V1) holds the most precise spatial representation, it has been suggested that motion-induced position shifts might be mediated by feedback from extra-striate motion areas such as area MT/V5 to V1 (Durant & Johnston,
2004; Nishida & Johnston,
1999).
A number of studies have looked at the cortical coding of the position of moving objects and the position of objects subject to motion drag. Fu, Shen, Gao, and Dan (
2004) demonstrated a shift in the responsivity function of cat primary visual cortical neurons in a direction opposite to that of a moving stimulus in their receptive field. They suggest that this could cause moving stimuli to be seen as advanced relative to the static responsivity profile of a cell's receptive field and, therefore, account for effects such as those shown by De Valois and De Valois (
1991). Sundberg, Fallah, and Reynolds (
2006) showed a similar effect in area V4 of the macaque monkey. In humans, Whitney et al. (
2003) showed that the retinotopic BOLD response in primary visual cortex (V1) shifts in a direction opposite to that of the motion of a set of drifting Gabors. These studies suggest that an object's motion can modulate the cortical coding of its position in low-level retinotopic areas, but that disassociations can exist between retinotopic activity and perceived position (Whitney et al.,
2003). However, subsequent data have suggested an alternative view of the putative shift in the retinotopic BOLD response in human V1 (Liu, Ashida, Smith, & Wandell,
2006). Less research has focused on the cortical coding of objects subject to motion drag, however, a recent study by Maus, Fischer, and Whitney (
2009) has suggested that a physical shift in position and a position shift induced by motion drag show corresponding shifts in retinotopic activity patterns in early visual areas, including V1.
Further evidence points to the role of MT/V5 in the etiology of the motion signal that modulates perceived position in both motion-induced position shifts and motion drag. Motion-defined contours that can only be seen at a global level, by pooling and segregating local motion signals and integrating over time, also appear shifted in position (Durant & Zanker,
2009). Position shifts in luminance modulated drifting random dot fields increase with motion coherence (Mussap & Prins,
2002). Motion drag of static spatially separate flashed objects depends on the perceived motion of a bistable stimulus (Shim & Cavanagh,
2004) and can also be produced by invisible occluded object motion (Watanabe, Sato, & Shimojo,
2003).
MT/V5 is known to respond to object and pattern motion and is strongly implicated in the mediation of our phenomenal experience of motion, including global motion stimuli, which require the integration of local motion signals (Born & Bradley,
2005; Newsome & Pare,
1988; Salzman, Britten, & Newsome,
1990; Salzman, Murasugi, Britten, & Newsome,
1992). McGraw, Walsh, and Barrett (
2004) looked at the effect of TMS over MT/V5 and V1 on the magnitude of position shift induced by adaptation to drifting Gabors. They found that TMS over MT/V5, but not V1, significantly reduced the magnitude of motion-induced position shift. From this, they suggest that the locus of the effect of motion on perceived position is in MT/V5 itself, rather than the result of feedback from MT/V5 down to V1. However, it remains possible that motion information from multiple levels in the visual system has an influence on perceived position. For example, anticipatory neural responses to motion have been observed at as low a level as the retina (Berry, Brivanlou, Jordan, & Meister,
1999). The focus of the present study was therefore in understanding the processing stage at which the motion signal that influences perceived position arises.