After the data cleaning based on oculomotor behavior, 84.56% of total trials were analyzed (see
General methods section). Participants responded correctly on 86.68% of the remaining trials (
SD = 6.65). Only correct responses were included in reaction time analyses. We performed an ANOVA with participants as a random variable and adaptor–target relation and adaptor duration as main factors. We found a main effect of relation, with shorter reaction times in related vs. unrelated trials [21 ms,
F(1,15) = 10.19,
p < 0.01], as well as a main effect of adaptor duration, with shorter reaction times in the short vs. the long duration condition [26 ms,
F(1,15) = 17.41,
p < 0.001]. Again, we found an interaction between relation and duration [
F(1,15) = 20.63,
p < 0.001], showing a priming effect in the short duration condition (53 ms,
t(15) = 4.89,
p < 0.01), while the habituation effect for long duration condition did not reach significance (−11 ms,
p > 0.1). Analyzing effects of sensory adaptation depending on adaptor visibility revealed that in the short duration condition, the priming effect increased with adaptor visibility, while in the long duration condition, the habituation effect was significant at low visibility and was abolished as soon as the adaptor became visible (see
Figure 5 and
Table 1). Finally, regarding subjective discriminability, an ANOVA with participants as random variable revealed that neither adaptor–target relation nor adaptor duration influenced adaptor discriminability (both
p > 0.1). To sum up, relying on subjective measures of discriminability with constant stimulation and a within-subject design,
Experiment 3 confirmed that the directionality of sensory adaptation (i.e., priming or habituation) depended both on perceptual awareness and adaptor duration. When perceptual awareness was absent, we found evidences for the reverse of priming into habituation, although the priming effect induced by short-lasting stimuli did not reach significance. In comparison to the other experiments, each trial here requested the participant to realize one task on the target followed by another task on the adaptor. This combination might have changed the participants' strategy and, therefore, disrupted the priming effect. Nonetheless, the effect amplitude (i.e., 26 ms) was similar to what we found in the other experiments, and it remains possible that the analysis we performed based on subjective visibility grouping simply lacked statistical power. Finally, when perceptual awareness was present, the decay of priming we found is similar to the rise-and-fall pattern previously observed during object priming (Zago et al.,
2005). Mechanistic interpretation of this pattern supports the succession of facilitation and selection. First, the processing of all stimulus features is facilitated, which explains the origin of priming effects. After longer exposure, only diagnostic features of the adaptor stimulus remain encoded, which decreases the similarity between the adaptor and the target and induces the decay of priming effects (Wiggs & Martin,
1998).