It is useful to consider the brain regions that could contribute to monocular cueing. Neurons that respond to only one of the eyes are found in the retina, lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus (LGN), V1 (Hubel & Wiesel,
1968) and the superficial layers of the superior colliculus. There are also a very small number of monocular cells in extra-striate visual areas (Hubel & Wiesel,
1968; Leopold & Logothetis,
1996; Zeki,
1978), however beyond these stages information about the eye-of-origin of the stimulus is lost. The main effects of a monocular cue revealed by our experiments were inhibition of monocular locations in the other eye combined with inhibition at different locations in the same eye. Suppression between the neuronal representations of locations in the same eye can occur as early as the retina. Retinal ganglion cells are inhibited by information outside their receptive field, and these suppressive effects could account for the inhibitory interactions between a cue and a subsequent target presented to the same eye (w
3 in
Figure 7) (Alitto & Usrey,
2008). These inhibitory interactions between monocular representations of the same eye are also found in the LGN (Alitto & Usrey,
2008; Webb, Tinsley, Vincent, & Derrington,
2005), which is at the same time the first stage where inhibitory interactions between different eyes can take place (w
1 and w
2). Inhibitory interactions between LGN neurons are partially mediated through the thalamic reticular nucleus that provides inhibition to the LGN relay cells (Crick,
1984; Singer,
1977). A recent study demonstrated that this inhibition plays a role in an endogenous cueing task (McAlonan et al.,
2008). This study reported that an endogenous cue that directs attention to a particular spatial location increases the response of LGN neurons with a receptive field at that location while suppressing the activity of the reticular neurons with a receptive field at the same location (McAlonan et al.,
2008). These results, taken together, indicate that the inhibitory interactions between monocular representations that underlie the cueing effects of the present study could well be mediated by the interactions between the LGN and the thalamic reticular nucleus. Future studies will, however, also have to consider the primary visual cortex as a potential site for monocular attentional effects (Li,
2002), because the monocular neurons of area V1 dedicated to the two eyes engage in various forms of inhibitory and excitatory interactions (Kato, Bishop, & Orban,
1981; Webb, Dhruv, Solomon, Tailby, & Lennie,
2005) and area V1 contributes to endogenous and exogenous shifts of visual attention (Knierim & Van Essen,
1992; Roelfsema,
2006; Roelfsema, Tolboom, & Khayat,
2007).