The 2AFC method we employed, in which accurate response feedback is given in each trial, is expected to produce a bias-free estimation of subjects' thresholds. Nevertheless, in view of previous results showing bias without feedback in crowding experiments (Solomon et al.,
2004), we tested for its presence by fitting psychometric functions to the data. To construct the fits, we considered the actual orientation of the target in each trial and calculated the percentage of correct responses for each of these orientations. The data were fit using a weighted logistic model (using the built-in “glmfit” function in MATLAB) to account for the different number of measurements in the different orientations. There was considerable within- and between-subjects variability in the observed bias. Local orientation had a significant effect in relation to the observed bias (
F(3,12) = 20.22,
p < 0.0001). The pattern was in agreement with that reported by Solomon et al. (
2004), with a significant repulsion from the oblique (±45°) flankers (45°: mean bias of −1.19°,
t(4) = 10.17,
p < 0.0001; −45°: mean bias of 1°,
t(4) = −3.26,
p = 0.03) and no bias from the cardinally oriented flankers (horizontal: −0.08° and vertical: 0.13°, both
p > 0.5). Interestingly, the biases from oblique flankers were, however, considerably smaller than those previously reported. We therefore reanalyzed our data to verify that the results reported above (with threshold estimated from the staircase reversals) correspond to a bias-free orientation sensitivity measure. To this end, we computed a sensitivity measure from the psychometric curves. We defined this sensitivity as the difference (in log units) between the point of subjective equality (the point on the curve that is equal to 50% discrimination—this is in fact the bias) and the point corresponding to an 80% performance level (the performance level used in the staircase computation). This estimate of sensitivity is, in essence, free of bias. The agreement between this estimate and the threshold derived from the staircase estimation was very high, with
r 2 = 0.81 (based on Pearson correlation). An analysis of the results described above, using the threshold estimated from the psychometric fits, confirmed the effects described in the previous sections: local orientation (
F(3,12) = 88.59,
p < 0.0001), with threshold elevations of 0.41, 0.29, and 0.07 log units for the ±45°, iso-oriented, and orthogonal flankers, respectively (post-hoc significance pattern unchanged); global orientation (
F(3,12) = 62.08,
p < 0.0001), with threshold elevations of 0.42, 0.39, 0.17, and 0.2 log units for −45°, 45°, horizontal, and vertical arrangements, respectively (post-hoc significance pattern unchanged); visual field (
F(7,28) = 0.98,
p > 0.1); the interaction between visual field and global arrangement (
F(21,84) = 4.13), with threshold elevations of 0.37, 0.29, 0.3, and 0.22 log units for radial, −45°, 45°, and tangential arrangements, respectively (post-hoc significance pattern unchanged).