Outliers in the reaction time data were defined as points lying ±3 SD from each individual participant's overall mean. Data from timed-out trials (≥5000 ms before a response was recorded), trials with outliers and trials with incorrect responses did not contribute to the reaction time analyses. Approximately 11% of the data were rejected under these criteria.
A three-way ANOVA was conducted on the remaining reaction time data, with target presence, stimulus type, and emotional valence serving as repeated-measures factors. Robust main effects were found for all three factors. Overall, target-present trials elicited faster responses ( M = 1570.12, SEM = 93.73) than target-absent trials ( M = 1820.02, SEM = 98.23), F(1,19) = 27.43, p < .001, η p 2 = .59, responses to abstract stimuli ( M = 1612.04, SEM = 94.95) were faster than responses to schematic faces ( M = 1778.10, SEM = 99.16), F(1,19) = 8.89, p < .01, η p 2 = .32, and responses to ‘angry’ search targets ( M = 1586.92, SEM = 94.18) were faster than those to ‘happy’ targets ( M = 1803.22, SEM = 93.45), F(1,19) = 75.84, p < .001, η p 2 = .80. The non-significant two-way interactions of target presence × stimulus type ( F(1,19) = .455, p > .05, η p 2 = .02), and target presence × ‘emotional valence’ ( F(1,19) = 1.97, p > .05, η p 2 = .09), and the three-way interaction ( F(1,19) = 2.69, p > .05, η p 2 = .12), indicated very little variation in the overall pattern of results.
The only qualification of lower-order effects was made by the two-way interaction of stimulus type and ‘emotional valence,’ (
F(1,19) = 6.85,
p < .05,
η p 2 = .26), which showed that, averaged over the two conditions of target presence, the ‘threat advantage’ for abstract stimuli (angry:
M = 1469.3,
SEM = 101.02. happy:
M = 1704.55,
SEM = 104.12) was approximately twice as great as that for schematic faces (angry:
M = 1754.79,
SEM = 10313. happy:
M = 1851.65,
SEM = 103.50). Complete results, including pairwise comparisons between the two levels of emotional valence, are displayed in
Figure 3. The greater effects for abstract stimuli were somewhat surprising, but find converging evidence with the observations of Horstmann and Bauland (
2006) and Horstmann, Borgstedt, and Heumann (
2006) who noted that stronger threat advantages tended to occur with less complex stimuli. These results strongly suggest that the search advantage has little to do with emotional valence, because an effect of equal, or indeed greater, amplitude can be elicited by abstract stimuli sharing the basic structural features as schematic faces.
We note that the discrepant angry target face showed a latency time of 1601 ms whereas the discrepant happy target face showed a latency time of 1766 ms. Öhman et al. (
2001), reported time latencies of 1,172 ms and 1,300 ms respectively. In general, reaction times were of the order of 450 ms slower than in Öhman et al.'s studies. This is most likely due to the use of irregular grids, as well to the use of ‘happy’ distractors instead of neutral ones.
Search accuracy was high overall. A 2 × 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA showed that the pattern of results generally mirrored those for Reaction Time, and there was no evidence of speed-accuracy tradeoffs. The results are summarized in
Table 1. Accuracy was better for ‘angry’ stimuli overall, and this trend was observed for all combinations of target presence and stimulus type. The significant three-way interaction indicating that the ‘threat advantage’ was particularly pronounced for abstract stimuli with the search target present (see
Table 1). The ‘emotional valence’ pairwise difference was not significant for schematic stimuli, although the trend was consistent with a threat advantage.