Admittedly, one should not expect material color constancy to be perfect. Moreover, there is a physical reason (referred to as the mismatch of metamers; Wyszecki & Stiles,
1982) for material color constancy to be somewhat imperfect (Logvinenko,
2009b,
2012a). How can, then, material color constancy be measured? Clearly, asymmetric color matching will not do. At first glance, an alternative might be color naming and/or color categorizing that have been used in color constancy studies (Granzier, Brenner, & Smeets,
2009; Hansen, Walter, & Gegenfurtner,
2007; Olkkonen, Hansen, & Gegenfurtner,
2009; Olkkonen, Witzel, Hansen, & Gegenfurtner,
2011; Smithson & Zaidi,
2004; Troost & de Weert,
1991; Uchikawa, Uchikawa, & Boynton,
1989). The rationale is that although illumination change alters the color appearance of, say, a red apple, it keeps belonging to the same color category—“red.” The problem with this method is that, first, it is not clear how to measure the color categories (more or less) precisely; second, there are large interindividual differences between subjects; and third, the illumination may affect the color categories themselves. Indeed, will what the observer means by “red” actually remain the same under daylight and under red light?