The term “crowding” was introduced by Stuart and Burian (
1962) to describe the detrimental influence of flankers on target identification, a phenomenon originally reported by Korte (
1923; see also Ehlers,
1936). Key properties of the effect that can be considered the hallmarks of crowding have been discovered (Levi,
2008). The distance between target and flanker stimuli required to induce crowding depends upon the eccentricity in the visual field, and this “critical spacing” is approximately a constant fraction of eccentricity (Bouma,
1970,
1973). Bouma (
1970,
1973) reported a critical spacing around half the eccentricity, a relation that is commonly referred to as Bouma's Law, although others find lower scaling values (e.g., Strasburger, Harvey, & Rentschler,
1991). Bouma's Law suggests that the underlying mechanism of crowding is tied to the cortical mapping of space, for example, the eccentricity scaling could reflect a fixed distance on cortex (Pelli,
2008; Pelli & Tillman,
2008). Further, the spatial extent of crowding has been found to be anisotropic; “interaction regions” or crowding zones are elliptical, with radial elongation, i.e., with the longer axis in the foveal direction (Chambers & Wolford,
1983; Toet & Levi,
1992). Crowding has a detrimental effect upon identification, over and above any effect on simple detection of targets in the periphery (Andriessen & Bouma,
1976), and is specific to peripheral vision—in the fovea, the critical distance between flankers and targets is proportional to target size and can be attributed to simple contrast masking (Levi, Hariharan, & Klein,
2002). The location of flankers has some influence on the strength of the effect. Specifically, a flanker with a greater eccentricity than the target leads to more crowding than a flanker at the same distance from the target but is situated between the target and the fovea (e.g., Banks, Larson, & Prinzmetal,
1979; Chambers & Wolford,
1983; Petrov, Popple, & McKee,
2007). Lastly, crowding does not depend upon the size of the gap between the edges of the target and a flanker. It depends upon the distance between the center of the target and the center of a flanker (Strasburger et al.,
1991) or the centroid of multiple flankers (Levi & Carney,
2009).