What is the source of this isotropic masking effect, which appears at low spatial frequencies and increases with the speed of the masker? We suggest two possibilities: one based on noise and one based on suppression. According to the noise account, the low-spatial-frequency-biased threshold elevation we observe may simply reflect broad “within-channel” noise arising due to excitation of isotropic magnocellular-like mechanisms (Meese & Hess,
2004). Alternatively, the suppression account holds that strong responses in transient (motion-driven) temporal channels may elicit an active suppression of sustained temporal mechanisms (such as our static tests; Cass & Alais,
2006; Cass, Alais et al.,
2009) and that the suppression is isotropic with respect to orientation (Cass & Alais,
2006). It is possible that the physiological mechanism underpinning this isotropic masking is related to cross-orientation suppression (Allison, Smith, & Bonds,
2001; Cass, Stuit et al.,
2009; DeAngelis, Robson, Ohzawa, & Freeman,
1992; Li, Thompson, Duong, Peterson, & Freeman,
2006; Meese & Holmes,
2007,
2010). With respect to the recent proposal that cross-orientation masking may arise pre-cortically (Cass & Alais,
2006; Li et al.,
2006; Meese & Baker,
2009; Meier & Carandini,
2002), the similarity between our monoptic and dichoptic masking results would suggest that the underlying mechanism is probably cortical (Allison et al.,
2001; Cass, Stuit et al.,
2009; Morrone, Burr, & Maffei,
1982). By contrast, recent findings indicate that isotropic adaptation-induced threshold elevation effects are purely monocular (Cass,
2010), suggesting an early pre-cortical locus, possibly LGN (Solomon, Peirce, Dhruv, & Lennie,
2004).