Here, participants often failed to remember color or orientation, as shown by the large proportion of random guesses. Moreover, errors for these features were largely independent for a single object. How do we reconcile this with the extensive literature showing that working memory is sensitive to the object-based structure of a display? (Delvenne & Bruyer,
2004,
2006; Luck & Vogel,
1997; Olson & Jiang,
2002; Xu,
2002). We suggest that these random guesses may arise from probabilistic failures of self-sustaining neural networks (Amit, Brunel, & Tsodyks,
1994; Hebb,
1949; Johnson, Spencer, Luck, & Schoner,
2008; Johnson, Spencer, & Schöner,
2009; Rolls & Deco,
2010). In this framework, working memory is the maintenance of perceptual representations in the absence of bottom-up sensory input. We suggest that increased item or information load may lead to detrimental effects on working memory due to an increased likelihood of representational failure. However, the representational unit of memory failure is not at the level of the object, at least for objects defined by color and orientation. Consider that successful visual working memory is the sustained activation of representations in the absence of bottom-up perceptual input. Since orientation and color are encoded by largely independent neurons during perception (i.e., separable feature dimensions; Cant, Large, McCall, & Goodale,
2008; Garner,
1974), these features may form largely independent self-sustaining representations and these representations may fail independently.
2 Critically, this
probabilistic feature-store account proposes that the degree of independence for features in working memory is determined by the degree of overlap in neural coding of the features. If a task requires maintenance of two features that are coded by overlapping populations of neurons, then their representations will overlap in memory and will not fail independently. This prediction was assessed in
Experiment 2.