A question of longstanding interest is whether first- and second-order motion are processed by the same neural mechanisms. While investigation of this question is still ongoing, converging evidence from psychophysics (Chubb & Sperling,
1989; Derrington & Badcock,
1985; Ledgeway & Smith,
1994; Nishida & Sato,
1995), neuroimaging (Ashida, Lingnau, Wall, & Smith,
2007), and neuropsychology (Vaina & Cowey,
1996; Vaina, Soloviev, Bienfang, & Cowey,
2000) indicates that motion perception is subserved by at least two, and perhaps more (Lu & Sperling,
1995,
2001), subsystems. Indeed, many differences in the perceptual properties of first- and second-order motion have already been described (e.g., Ledgeway & Hutchinson,
2005; Nishida,
1993; Schofield & Georgeson,
2003). For example, while first-order motion can generate both static and dynamic motion aftereffects (MAEs), second-order motion has only been shown to generate dynamic MAE (Derrington & Badcock,
1985; Ledgeway,
1994). Additionally, longer presentation durations are required to perceive direction of second-order motion, typically not less than 120 ms versus approximately 20 ms for first-order motion (Cropper & Derrington,
1994; Derrington, Badcock, & Henning,
1993; Ledgeway & Hess,
2002). More recent work has found that the spatial tuning of near-threshold first- and second- order motion stimuli, as measured by minimum and maximum windows for spatial summation, differs considerably (Hutchinson & Ledgeway,
2010). The aim of the present work is to provide further comparative investigation of spatial properties of first- and second-order motion processing.