However, because RDS actually contain both binocular cues, it is possible that changing disparity contributes to speed sensitivity in conjunction with IOVD, but is not a sufficient cue in isolation. It is possible to design stimuli which attempt to isolate IOVD by removing the disparity signal, either by using uncorrelated dot patterns in each eye (e.g., Brooks,
2002; Shioiri et al.,
2000), or anticorrelated stimuli in which the luminance of the dots is reversed in each monocular half-image (e.g., Czuba, Rokers, Huk, & Cormack,
2010; Harris & Rushton,
2003; Rokers, Cormack, & Huk,
2008). However, detection performance for motion direction in uncorrelated patterns does not reach 100% even at high contrast (Shioiri et al.,
2000). Another problem with uncorrelated dot patterns is that there is the possibility of spurious disparity matching between individual dots in the uncorrelated monocular images. Although there is also the possibility of spurious coherent motion signals in DRDS, the large number of frames (e.g., 120 Hz display rate in our experiments) makes it much less likely than spatial matching in uncorrelated RDS, which have only two images, one for each eye (Harris et al.,
2008). Nefs and Harris (
2010) have shown that observers generally do not perceive displacement in two-frame motion displays containing only IOVD, although they can for displays with only changing disparity. Similarly, motion in depth in anticorrelated displays is typically perceived without any corresponding change in position (Rokers et al.,
2008). In addition, the elimination of the percept of depth in anticorrelated dot patterns does not guarantee that there is no neural response from disparity detectors (Cumming & Parker,
1997; Harris & Rushton,
2003). Thus it is more difficult to isolate the IOVD cue than changing disparity, which is why many experiments rely on the comparison between RDS and DRDS.