In humans and honeybees, the spatial resolution of the color (chromatic) channel is lower than that of the luminance (achromatic) channel (Giurfa, Vorobyev, Brandt, Posner, & Menzel,
1997; Mullen,
1985). Bees, for instance, can detect flowers providing achromatic contrast to the background from a much larger distance than flowers only providing chromatic contrast (Giurfa et al.,
1997). Likewise, there are indications that birds use achromatic cues to discriminate fine details (Osorio, Miklósi, & Gonda,
1999) and to distinguish between simple visual textures (Jones & Osorio,
2004).
Interestingly, birds are believed to have photoreceptors almost completely specialized for either achromatic or chromatic vision. Humans and bees use the majority of their receptors (95% of the cones in humans (Wyszecki & Stiles,
1982) and the L-receptors in all ommatidia in bees (Srinivasan & Lehrer,
1988)) for achromatic vision, providing high resolution. Quite differently, research so far suggests that birds use only about half of the entire cone population—the double cones—for achromatic vision (e.g., Campenhausen & Kirschfeld,
1998; Goldsmith & Butler,
2003,
2005; Osorio et al.,
1999; Vorobyev & Osorio,
1998; reviewed in Martin & Osorio,
2008) and while humans and bees use the same receptors also for color vision (Vorobyev, Brandt, Peitsch, Laughlin, & Menzel,
2001; Wyszecki & Stiles,
1982), birds are thought to use the remaining half of their cones—single cones of four different spectral sensitivities—for color vision (e.g., Goldsmith & Butler,
2003,
2005; Osorio et al.,
1999; Vorobyev & Osorio,
1998; reviewed in Martin & Osorio,
2008).
The color discrimination abilities in birds have been studied in depth (reviewed in Kelber, Vorobyev, & Osorio,
2003; Martin & Osorio,
2008) but little is known about the relation between the achromatic and chromatic channels, and nothing is known about the spatial resolution of the chromatic channel. This lack of knowledge limits our understanding of how birds perceive color patterns, such as those of plumages and eggs, which are believed to be vital signals of fitness and identity (Bennett & Théry,
2007; Kilner,
2006).
A robust measure of the spatial resolution of a visual channel is the spatial contrast sensitivity function (CSF; De Valois & De Valois,
1990). Not only does this function allow for an estimation of acuity, it also describes to what spatial frequencies the visual channel is most sensitive. However, while the spatial contrast sensitivity of the achromatic channel has been determined for a number of species (De Valois & De Valois,
1990; Ghim & Hodos,
2006; Uhlrich, Essock, & Lehmkuhle,
1981), the corresponding sensitivity in the color channel is only known for humans (Mullen,
1985).
In this study, we present the results from three experiments with budgerigars (
Melopsittacus undulatus). First, we determined the CSF function for achromatic gratings. Second, we determined the CSF for isoluminant chromatic gratings. This was possibly since budgerigars have photoreceptors with known spectral sensitivities (Bowmaker, Heath, Wilkie, & Hunt,
1997; Goldsmith & Butler,
2003,
2005; Lind & Kelber,
2009), which allowed us to precisely adjust the stimuli so that single cones could be tested separately from the double cones. This is the first description of a CSF for the color channel in any animal besides humans (Mullen,
1985).
It is known that in humans, the CSF is higher for red–green gratings than for blue–yellow gratings, possibly because blue-sensitive cones are relatively scarce within the human retina. Similarly, in budgerigars, the short-wavelength-sensitive (SWS) cones are less abundant (ca. 10–12% of total cone population) than medium-wavelength-sensitive (MWS) cones (ca. 19–21%) and long-wavelength-sensitive (LWS) cones (ca. 16–20%; Hart,
2001; Wilkie et al.,
1998). For these reasons, we determined the CSFs for both red–green and blue–green gratings, both types isoluminant to double cones.
While these tests serve to investigate the spatial tuning in either the achromatic or the chromatic visual channels in budgerigars, most naturally occurring stimuli provide contrast to both (De Valois & De Valois,
1990). Therefore, we performed a third experiment, in which we examined the detection of gratings with a constant low achromatic contrast but with variable red–green chromatic contrast.