Crowding might make it impossible to individuate dots and bind them with their locations, conceivably leaving the participant with only an estimate of the proportion of each color on each surface. Note however that this would be an advance in the understanding of the effects of crowding (for potentially related crowding literature, see Dakin, Cass, Greenwood, & Bex,
2010; Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, & Morgan,
2001). In the present experiment, a role for crowding was tested by manipulating dot density (12, 48, or 96 dots/surface). The average nearest neighbor distances of dots for the 12, 48, and 96 dots/surface conditions were 0.78, 0.38, and 0.26 deg among all dots in the display, and 1.15, 0.54, and 0.38 among dots moving in the same direction. While 48 and 96 dots/surface conditions were highly crowded, many dots in the central 1.6 deg of the display in the 12 dots/surface condition were not crowded, according to Bouma's (
1970) law that the critical spacing for crowding to occur is about half the eccentricity of an item. Note that this estimate is quite conservative, because it has been shown that in the isoeccentric direction (two dots at same eccentricity but different directions), the critical spacing is much smaller, by a factor of two or three (Toet & Levi,
1992). For the five participants,
Figure 3 shows the average performance for the color switch detection task. Even when only 12 dots/surface were used so that many dots would not have been crowded based on nearest neighbor distance calculation (Pelli & Tillman,
2008), performance was still poor when there was no accompanying surface color change.