Behavioral studies, on the other hand, have demonstrated that the processing of a facial feature (e.g., eye, nose, mouth, …) is affected by alterations to the identity or the position of one or several other features of the individual face (e.g., Farah, Wilson, Drain, & Tanaka,
1998; Homa, Haver, & Schwartz,
1976; Mermelstein, Banks, & Prinzmetal,
1979; Sergent,
1984; Suzuki & Cavanagh,
1995; Tanaka & Farah,
1993; Tanaka & Sengco,
1997; Young, Hellawell, & Hay,
1987). The most compelling illustration of this phenomenon comes from an adaptation of the composite face effect (Young et al.,
1987) to create a visual illusion in which identical top halves of faces are perceived as being slightly different if they are aligned with different bottom parts, even when the bottom parts are irrelevant and not attended to (
Figure 1). This composite face illusion (CFI) is a particularly clear demonstration that the features of a face (here the two halves of a single face) cannot be perceived in isolation. That is, the perception of the attended top part depends on the identity of the bottom part and its position (since the illusion vanishes when the two parts are misaligned spatially; for empirical demonstrations in face matching tasks, e.g., Goffaux & Rossion,
2006; Hole,
1994; Le Grand, Mondloch, Maurer, & Brent,
2004; Michel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara,
2006; Rossion & Boremanse,
2008). This empirical observation is generally interpreted as evidence for the integration of the facial features into a Gestalt, a global picture, a “configural” (Sergent,
1984; Young et al.,
1987), or a “holistic” (Farah et al.,
1998; Tanaka & Farah,
1993) representation.