In this study, we have successfully biased participants' perceived direction of the motion quartet by manipulating the eye-of-origin of separate spatial halves of each motion quartet. This finding suggests a monocular over dichoptic advantage in the perception of apparent motion. This result is consistent with previous binocular rivalry studies where it has been demonstrated that eye-of-origin information contributes to overall percept (Blake,
2001; Hsieh, Colas, &
Kanwisher, 2012; Ooi & He,
1999; Shimojo & Nakayama,
1990). In the processing of motion signals, behavioral studies have demonstrated that both monocular and binocular motion components are present. In addition, the proportion of monocular and binocular motion processing varies in different cortical regions (McColl & Mitchell,
1998; Raymond,
1993; Wade, Swanston, & De Weert,
1993). This variation is evidenced by different degrees of interocular transfer of motion aftereffects depending on stimulus parameters (McColl & Mitchell,
1998; Raymond,
1993; Wade et al.,
1993). Indeed, our current finding supports the notion that there is a monocular motion component. Furthermore, our findings would suggest that monocular motion signals could exert significant influence over the final motion percept.
Whereas our study does not address where eye-specific modulation occurs, it is possible that V1 is the site of eye-specific modulation. Although it is known that MT/V5 is the “motion processing” brain area, eye-of-origin information is lost (Dubner & Zeki,
1971; Watson et al.,
1993; Zeki et al.,
1991). However, MT/V5 is highly interconnected to other regions of the cortex and integrates a large number of inputs from both cortical and subcortical pathways (Born & Bradley,
2005; McCool & Britten,
2008; Wurtz, McAlonan, Cavanaugh, & Berman,
2011). Importantly, it has been suggested that signals originating from V1 are strongest (Born & Bradley,
2005) and correlate with visual perceptions (Hsieh, Caplovitz, & Tse,
2006; Hsieh, Vul, & Kanwisher,
2010; Hsieh & Tse,
2009,
2010a,
2010b; Tong,
2003; Troncoso et al.,
2007). Since eye-of-origin information is preserved in early visual areas (Connolly & Van Essen,
1984; Horton et al.,
1990), and recurrent connections between V1 and extrastriate areas are likely required for visual awareness (Friston,
2005; Pascual-Leone & Walsh,
2001; Tong,
2003), we suggest that V1 is a candidate region where eye-specific modulation occurs.
Another explanation that can account for our results is different neuronal interactions in the horizontal and vertical eye specific-bias condition. It has been documented that stimuli presented outside of a neuron's receptive field can modulate how a stimulus presented within the receptive field is processed. The “peripheral effect” and “shift effect” are well-documented examples of long-range interactions between the retina and LGN (Fischer, Barth, & Sternheim,
1978; Krüger & Fischer,
1973; Levick, Oyster, & Davis,
1965; McIlwain,
1964). Therefore, it is possible that each eye-specific bias condition elicited different long-range interaction, giving rise to different proportions of vertical and horizontal motion percept.
Our finding is consistent with that of Ammons and Weitz (
1951) in which they demonstrated that dichoptic presentation resulted in less frequent perception of apparent motion when compared to monocular presentation. However, the difference across the two presentation conditions in that study could be due to differences in their experimental set up, in which a septum with graduated wings was used to restrict each eye's vision during dichoptic presentation of the apparent motion display and was not used during monocular presentation. In a setup like this, the stimulus perceived in one eye may rival with the septum perceived in the other eye, and therefore interfere with apparent motion perception.
In a similar experiment by Shipley and colleagues (
1945), it was determined that under the right conditions, dichoptic is not significantly different from monocular stimulation when perceiving apparent motion (Shipley, Kenney, & King,
1945). This null finding is contrary to what we have demonstrated in our experiment. We suspect that Shipley and colleagues (
1945) did not find a difference between monocular and dichoptic stimulation because each subject in their study completed only eight trials, which is significantly fewer in contrast to ours (a total of 8 × 40 trials for each eye bias condition). It is therefore possible that the earlier study lacked statistical power to tease apart differences in monocular versus dichoptic presentation of apparent motion. The difference in findings could also be attributed to differences in experimental paradigms employed: In our experiment, monocular and dichoptic apparent motion were presented simultaneously whilst Shipley and colleagues (
1945) chose to present monocular and dichoptic stimuli separately.
By manipulating the direction of apparent motion presented to each eye and measuring proportion of each perceived motion direction, we were able to quantify the effect exerted by each monocular channel on the final motion percept. A unique feature of our motion quartet display is that both monocular and dichoptic apparent motion were made to compete directly with each other for perceptual awareness. The final perceived motion direction would directly indicate the relative strength of monocular versus dichoptic apparent motion presentation. In the experiment described here, we have effectively shown the advantage of monocular over dichoptic stimulation in viewing apparent motion.