Here, we employed an adaptation protocol to examine the spread of attentional modulation. The basic logic is the following: if attending to a specific feature selectively increases the activity of neurons preferring that feature, those neurons will become more adapted, as neural adaptation tends to increase with stronger neuronal response (e.g., Gardner et al.,
2005; Sclar, Lennie, & DePriest,
1989). Behaviorally, such selective adaptation should lead to perceptual aftereffects due to unbalanced activity from neuronal subpopulations (Clifford,
2002). Thus, it has been shown that attending to one direction in a compound motion stimulus produces a motion aftereffect (Lankheet & Verstraten,
1995), and attending to an orientation in a compound grating stimulus produces a tilt aftereffect (Liu, Larsson, & Carrasco,
2007; Spivey & Spirn,
2000). These previous studies only tested the aftereffect at the adapter location; here, we used attention-induced tilt aftereffect to measure the spread of orientation-based attention across the visual field. Furthermore, we tested tilt aftereffect in remote locations that were never stimulated, which allowed us to obtain a pure measure of the spread of feature-based attention without local sensory adaptation. This technique has been demonstrated in previous studies on attention to motion (Arman et al.,
2006; Liu & Mance,
2011). Finally, because the tilt aftereffect has been linked to neuronal population responses in V1 (e.g., Dragoi, Sharma, & Sur,
2000; Jin, Dragoi, Sur, & Seung,
2005), our psychophysical measurement can be considered to index attentional modulation in V1.