Understanding the potential role of adaptation to optical aberrations is important because these aberrations can be and are routinely altered in a variety of ways. Certain treatments such as refractive surgery induce significant amounts of optical aberrations (Marcos, Barbero, Llorente, & Merayo-Lloves,
2001), while optical aids such as progressive spectacles produce significant amounts of astigmatism and field distortions (Villegas, Alcon, & Artal,
2006). Thus, how observers respond to these corrections may depend importantly on how they are able to neuronally adapt to these optical changes. Several ocular pathologies also alter the natural wave aberration of the eye (for example, keratoconus, which produces a progressive deformation of the cornea and an increase in the HOA of the eye; Barbero et al.,
2001). Sabesan and Yoon (
2009) reported that keratoconic eyes do not achieve the visual benefit expected by the optical improvement and suggested that long-term adaptation to poor retinal image quality may limit the visual improvement immediately following correction. Conversely, they found better visual performance in real keratoconic eyes than normal eyes with a keratoconus wave aberration (simulated by Adaptive Optics), despite a similar optical degradation in both cases, which they attributed to adaptation to HOA in the keratoconic eyes (Sabesan & Yoon,
2010). Prior adaptation to the blur imposed by the optics was also attributed as the basis for discrepancies between predictions of optical performance under combined aberrations (astigmatism and coma) and visual performance in habitually uncorrected astigmats (de Gracia et al.,
2010; de Gracia, Dorronsoro, Marin, Hernández, & Marcos,
2011). In addition, habitually uncorrected astigmats appeared more insensitive to the induction of astigmatism (with all low- and high-order aberrations corrected with adaptive optics) than non-astigmatic or normally corrected astigmatic subjects. In all previous examples, the fact that subjects with the same optical aberrations (achieved by manipulation of the wave aberration pattern with adaptive optics) exhibit very different relative visual performance to changes in the optics suggests that prior visual experience plays an important role in the visual response. Alternatively, the ultimate goal of refractive correction is the elimination of HOA of the eye. Debate is ongoing whether patients adapt to their new pattern of optical aberrations so that vision is less compromised than the optical degradation of their retinal image quality would suggest, if aberrations have been induced, or conversely, whether they can take advantage of an improved image quality if aberrations have been corrected. For example, Marcos, Sawides, Gambra, and Dorronsoro (
2008) and Rossi and Roorda (
2010) showed, in most cases, an immediate improvement of visual acuity upon correction of high-order aberrations that showed a minimal effect of short-term adaptation or perceptual learning. However, as noted by the authors, their study did not assess the potential effects of adaptation on subjective image quality or on visual acuity or sensitivity to natural images. Thus, the potential impact of adaptation on refractive corrections remains unknown. In addition, we found in a subjective image sharpness assessment experiment that observers chose as “the sharpest image” 84% on average of the images seen through a full correction of their HOA (Sawides, Gambra et al.,
2010). The correction of HOA produces, therefore, a clear increase of the subjective impression of sharpness. However, the question remains open whether the sharpest image actually appears “too sharp” to the subject because they are adapted to compensate for their aberrations. In these prior studies, adaptation to HOA was only implicitly tested by asking how perceived image quality or acuity changed with a change in the aberration pattern. In the current study, we instead directly tested whether subjects can adapt to changes in the magnitude of HOA, by measuring the aftereffects of exposure to different levels of HOA on subjective image focus. We used a similar paradigm to that used by Webster et al. (
2002) to study aftereffects following adaptation to blur or sharpened images. However, rather than artificial symmetric blur (introduced by filtering the image), we tested for potential aftereffects after adaptation to various levels of blur produced by actual HOA centered around the actual magnitudes of blur that the observers were normally exposed to (simulated in the image while subjects viewed the stimuli with their native HOA corrected with AO). Subjects were exposed to their own aberrations as well as other subject's aberrations. We also explored the dependence of the effect on the amount of blur in the adapting image, as well as the transfer of the effect across different adapting and test images.