The AOSLO was set to record a high-resolution 512 × 512 pixel video of a 2° square patch of the retina with cellular-level resolution (Roorda et al.,
2002). High resolution was obtained by using adaptive optics, which dynamically measures and corrects for the ocular aberrations of the eye during imaging (Liang, Williams, & Miller,
1997). To correct for eye motion, we implemented a set of techniques whereby we tracked the retina in real time by analyzing the distortions within each scanned image (Arathorn et al.,
2007; Sheehy et al.,
2012; Yang et al.,
2010) and placed the stimulus at a targeted retinal location within each frame. The eye tracking was accomplished by acquiring each image as a series of narrow 3.75 arc min (16 pixel) strips or subimages and cross-correlating the strips against a reference frame, which was acquired earlier in the imaging session. The X- and Y-displacements of each strip that were required to conform it to the reference frame were a direct measure of the eye motion that occurred during each frame. Visible stimuli were presented to the subject by modulating the laser beam as it scanned across the retina, much in the same way as an image is projected onto the surface of a cathode-ray-tube television screen (Poonja, Patel, Henry, & Roorda,
2005). To stabilize the stimulus, we used the eye position signal to guide the timing for the modulation of the scanning laser in order to place it at a targeted location. The original intended use of this capability was to stabilize a stimulus on a particular cone or area regardless of eye motion for neurophysiological experimentation (e.g., Sincich, Zhang, Tiruveedhula, Horton, & Roorda,
2009) or for targeted visual function testing (e.g., Tuten, Tiruveedhula, & Roorda,
2012). But because this stabilization is determined from computed eye motion, other stimulus motions relative to the retinal mosaic can be computed and delivered with high precision. We refer to it as a
retina-contingent retinal display. The motion of the stimulus and/or the red field that was delivered directly to the retina with the AOSLO was controlled with a direction and a magnitude that were a computed function of the eye's own motion, according to
gain and
angle. The gain refers to the speed of the motion applied to the stimulus relative to the actual eye motion. The angle refers to the direction of the motion relative to the actual eye motion at any given moment. For example, a gain of 1 and an angle of 0 generates the stabilized stimulus condition because the stimulus is moved an amount equal to the motion of the eye (gain 1) in a direction that is the same as the eye motion (angle 0). A gain of 0 refers to a stimulus that is fixed relative to the field but one that naturally slips across the retina in a direction that is opposite to the eye's motion. A gain of 1 with an angle of 180 doubles the retinal slip of the stimulus caused by eye motion, and a gain of 2 and an angle of 0 mean that the stimulus will move across the retina ahead of eye motion. Other angles will cause the stimulus to move with components that are not in line with the eye's motion. Regardless of angle, any gain greater than 0 produces relative motion between the target and the fixation cross.