The stimuli were presented in a dark room on two 14-in. Sony monitors controlled by visual attenuators (Institute for Sensory Research, Syracuse University) and the Video Toolbox software (Pelli & Zhang,
1991; Brainard,
1997) running on an Apple G4 computer. The monitor resolution was set at 800 by 600 pixels with a frame rate of 100 Hz. Test and mask stimuli were presented on different monitors with the background luminance at 25 cd/m
2. These images were combined by a half-transparent front-surface mirror, such that the visible contrast of the stimuli was half of that on the screen. All data in this study are specified in terms of the visible contrast of the combined image. The stimuli were presented to observer's right eye at a distance of 136 cm, while the left eye was covered by an eye patch.
The range of spatial frequency channels was evaluated at three eccentricities: 2°, 4°, and 8°. For 2° and 4° the fixation cross was located in the center of the screen and the test could appear randomly on either side, as shown in
Figure 2. In this case a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) task was used: the observer had to say on which side the test was presented. In the case of the 8° eccentricity condition, the screen was too small for such a bilateral presentation. In this condition the fixation was set beyond the edge of the screen and the mask and the mask+test stimuli were shown at the screen center in the two-interval forced-choice (2IFC) procedure. Beginning of the 2IFC presentation intervals were indicated by brief (100 ms) sounds.
The mask was an oblique sinusoidal grating tilted by 10° to the vertical and filled the whole screen. For the 2° and 4° eccentricity conditions it had a small blank field in the center, where the fixation cross was shown. The test stimulus, depending on whether the higher or lower channels were measured, had either an elliptical Gabor or elliptical Gaussian luminance profiles (
Figure 2). Test and mask were presented synchronously with a raised cosine temporal profile with a 3-s epoch. The Gaussian profile had a fixed height and a width that varied with eccentricity, as specified by the formula:
where
L0 is the background luminance,
c is the contrast parameter,
p is the test eccentricity on a given trial,
σ is the Gaussian width parameter computed based on
Equation 2b from the spatial frequency
f probed by the Gaussian bar,
d is the test eccentricity, and
T − 3 s is the presentation epoch. When the test had a Gabor profile, the envelope was vertically elongated by a factor of 2 and the cosine bars were vertical:
A Gaussian test superimposed with a mask is illustrated on the left panel of
Figure 2, while the right panel illustrates the Gabor test. It should be noted that this Gaussian test example is close to the widest employed in the experiments.
An experiment consisted of a series of measurements varying mask spatial frequency for a fixed test stimulus. Each series began with a measurement of the test detection contrast with no mask present. Then, the test contrast was set at twice the detection threshold, and the mask threshold contrast was measured for spatial frequency values of the mask sampled in half-octave steps. Sometimes, probably because of observer fatigue, the detection threshold increased during an experimental series to a level at which observer stopped reliably seeing the test alone for extended periods of time. In such cases the whole routine was terminated and started over with a new measurement of the detection threshold. We encouraged observers to take frequent rests to avoid such problematic events.
All measurements were conducted with the three-down/one-up nonparametric adaptive method (Levitt,
1971), which converges at 75% correct. Every measurement was repeated at least three times, and the results were averaged. In cases in which the maximum contrast mask did not bring the test to threshold, we depicted the tuning function as if the mask contrast was 50% at that point (a maximum value, since the two screens were mixed with half-transparent mirror) to express the widest tuning function compatible with the failure to reach threshold. We did not employ a Bayesian adaptive method because there were no data on the value and stability of the psychometric slope in the masking sensitivity task. Also, the relatively high miss error level in this task would have dramatically reduced the efficacy of the adaptive methods. A potential complication with the masking sensitivity paradigm might be the susceptibility of the masking sensitivity results to fluctuations in the contrast detection threshold, which are known to be dependent, for instance, on the time of day and the blood glucose level (Barlow, Khan, & Farell,
2003). To address this issue, we varied mask spatial frequency in both in ascending and descending order and averaged their masking sensitivities in logarithmic space. Such a procedure removes any directional bias on the shape of the masking sensitivity curves.
The experiments were conducted with one of the authors and two paid observers: all male, 14 to 44 years old, with normal vision without correction. Two of them were experienced observers who had participated in numerous psychophysical experiments; for the third it was the first psychophysical experiment. Two observers were naïve about the goal of the study, the other being the first author of the paper.