In the second set of experiments, we tested whether binocular rivalry with our new stimulus can elicit the spontaneous alternation of OKN in correspondence with the perceptual alternation. All participants (
n = 5) who observed our new binocular rivalry stimulus experienced the fluctuation of awareness between the left and right eye images, as expected. Furthermore, although the contrast polarity of our stimulus alternated every half-cycle (i.e., 120 ms in our experiments) due to the phase-shift operation (see
Figure 1), a single phase of perceptual dominance usually lasted for several seconds, thus spanning multiple alternations of contrast.
Figure 3 shows the behavioral results recorded from three participants during their observation of the binocular rivalry stimulus. As in
Experiment 1, blue lines in the first row indicate the eye position over time, and the black dashed lines are the button press reports about the dominantly perceived image. OKN was elicited even by the dichoptic presentation of opponent phase-shift motions, and the direction of the slow-phase movement of OKN flipped when the dominantly perceived image had spontaneously flipped. The orange lines in the second row of
Figure 3, indicating eye velocity, also show the flip of the eye movement direction consistent with the perceptual alternation of the image in awareness. To quantify how well the flip of OKN corresponded to that of visual perception, we applied the same filtering analysis as in the first experiment and calculated the matching index between the actual button press reports of the dominant image and those reproduced from filtered eye movement responses. Red lines in the third row of
Figure 3 are the output of the filtering, which corresponds very well with the button press responses.
Figure 4A shows the matching index between the participants' reports and those reproduced from eye movements from all five participants. The left bar is the condition when the two images moved in the temporonasal direction, while the right bar is the condition of the nasotemporal direction. The mean matching index was 91.7% and 95.4% for the temporonasal and nasotemporal conditions, respectively (the mean of two conditions was 93.6%), and their difference was not statistically different (paired
t test:
p = 0.168). We also calculated the correlation coefficient between the button press reports and the filtered eye movement responses and found that the correlation was also quite high for the two conditions (0.94 for temporonasal condition and 0.95 for nasotemporal condition). There was no statistically significant effect of face orientation (upright vs. inverted) on the duration of the dominant phase (mean ± std: 3.489 ± 2.290 s vs. 3.353 ± 2.248 s, paired
t test:
p = 0.106) and on the reproduction of perceptual alternation from eye movement (92.5% vs. 93.2%, paired
t test:
p = 0.453). However, we observed a slight but statistically significant difference (paired
t test:
p = 0.045) in the matching index of the reproduction if we compared the condition when the recorded eye's (the left eye) motion was nasotemporal motion (leftward motion) and OKN followed the nasotemporal motion (96.6%) with the condition when the recorded eye's (the left eye) motion was temporonasal motion (rightward motion) and OKN followed the temporonasal motion (88.7%). The poorer performance in the temporonasal condition could be accounted for by the idea that alternation of OKN that corresponds to the perceptual alternation in rivalry is driven by the cortical motion signal, while the temporonasal motion signal that directly passes through subcortical areas interferes with the rivalry-dependent modulation of the cortical motion signal. The best matching index and/or correlation coefficient between the button press reports and the filtered eye movement responses was obtained when the time lag of 448.2 ms (std = 74.9 ms) was introduced. This means that the flip of the OKN tends to precede button press reports. It is noteworthy that the time delay between the flip of the OKN and the button press response is larger in binocular rivalry condition than the time delay in
Experiment 1, in which the participants were expected to press the button as soon as possible when monocular inputs were physically switched. One possible explanation for the extended time delay is that the flip of perception in the binocular rivalry condition may be triggered by the eye movement change. Another explanation is that making the decision to report the perceptual flip is delayed because the perceptual flip does not necessarily occur instantaneously, but the perceived image is sometimes ambiguous: For some periods, participants experienced the mosaic patch-wise mixture of the two eyes' images, which made it difficult to report either eye's image as dominant. Regardless of the cause of the precedence of the OKN changes, the results indicate that OKN flips its direction depending on what the participants see rather than whether monocular inputs were physically flipped or dichoptically presented. Therefore, OKN provides an objective behavioral index of which one of the two eyes' images is perceptually dominant at a time.