Although this work reveals that scission can induce large changes in the chromatic appearance of a target, there is currently no general theory that specifies the chromatic and geometric conditions that support color scission. Despite a number of noteworthy exceptions (cf. Chen & D'Zmura,
1998; D'Zmura, Colantoni, Knoblauch, & Laget,
1997; Faul & Ekroll,
2002; Khang & Zaidi,
2002), the vast majority of research into perceived transparency has used achromatic stimuli and focused on conditions that support “balanced” transparency (i.e., conditions in which the transparent surface has a uniform transmittance and color). However, our laboratory introduced a class of achromatic displays that evoke percepts in which the transparent layer appears to vary continuously in opacity (Anderson,
1999,
2003; Anderson & Winawer,
2005,
2008; see
Figure 2). The displays were constructed from noise patterns with an amplitude spectrum of 1/
f2. The separate frequency components were summed with random phases and orientations, producing a texture with a cloud-like appearance (
Figure 2). A number of constraints were found to be critical in predicting when scission occurred with targets formed by these textures, and how lightness and opacity was assigned to the resulting layers. First, for a coherent percept of scission to occur, the contrast polarity separating the target and its surround must have a constant sign. This polarity constraint is critical in eliciting both balanced (e.g., Adelson & Anandan,
1990; Anderson,
1997; Metelli,
1974) and unbalanced transparencies (Anderson,
1999,
2003; Anderson & Winawer,
2005,
2008). The direction of the contrast change determines how lightness is partitioned between the two layers: if the surround is darker than the target region, the target appears light; if the surround is lighter than the target, the target appears dark. The second critical constraint involves how opacity and lightness are assigned to the multiple layers in conditions where the polarity constraint is satisfied. It was shown that the perceived transmittance of the cloud is determined by how the strength of the edge (i.e., its contrast) varies along the target–surround boundary (Anderson,
1999; Anderson & Winawer,
2005,
2008). The transparent surface appears most dense in the regions along the target–surround boundary that generates the lowest contrast (i.e., the weakest edge strength). In the limit where the contrast of the target–surround border goes to zero, the near surface appears opaque. At the other end of the opacity scale, it was found that the highest contrast contour segment along the target–surround boundary appears in plain view, i.e., unobscured by any transparent or opaque surface. This constraint was observed for all values of contrast tested and, hence, appears as a general “anchor point” for perceived opacity. For this reason, this constraint was dubbed a
transmittance anchoring principle (TAP), which states that the visual system treats the highest contrast regions along continuous contours as “anchor” points in scaling the transmittance of a transparent layer (by assigning a transmittance value of 1 to these regions, causing the underlying surface to appear in plain view). It should be noted that this anchor point is in no way mandated by the physics of transparency. Rather, it expresses a bias of the visual system to only postulate the existence of a transparent layer when there is some reduction in contrast magnitude of same polarity edges that is best “explained” by the presence of transparent media.