For faces, cars, chessboards, fingerprints, etc., perceptual expertise with different object categories is associated with holistic processing (Bilalić, Langner, Ulrich, & Grodd,
2011; Bukach, Phillips, & Gauthier,
2010; Busey & Vanderkolk,
2005; Chase & Simon,
1973; Gauthier, Curran, Curby, & Collins,
2003; Richler, Tanaka, Brown, & Gauthier,
2008). Holistic processing is typically defined as either the obligatory attention to all parts of an object (Hole,
1994; Richler, Tanaka et al.,
2008) or the sensitivity to the detailed spatial relationships between parts (Maurer, LeGrand, & Mondloch,
2002). One common understanding is that perceptual expertise typically involves fine, subordinate-level discrimination between highly similar objects (e.g., differentiating between faces of two siblings or between two fingerprints), and during the acquisition of expertise, holistic processing develops as an optimal way to fulfill the demand of fine discrimination (Bukach, Bub, Gauthier, & Tarr,
2006). This claim has been supported by the association between identification performance and holistic processing of faces (Richler, Cheung, & Gauthier,
2011; but see Konar, Bennett, & Sekuler,
2009). Furthermore, training studies with novel objects have demonstrated that holistic processing develops after training that requires subordinate-level individuation, but not following training that involves coarser, basic-level categorization of objects (Nishimura & Maurer,
2008; A. C.-N. Wong, Palmeri, & Gauthier,
2009).