Previous studies on the effect of higher-order aberrations on letter-based visual acuity have tested the size threshold for recognition. Using logMAR acuity charts rendered with a constant root-mean-square (rms) amplitude of the first 12 Zernike modes (second to fourth radial order), Applegate et al. (
2002) showed that Zernike modes concentrated near the center of the pyramid (see
Figure 1) caused more disruption to visual acuity than those at the edges. Furthermore, they showed that the relationship between the amplitude of the aberration and the decrease in logMAR acuity was linear with slopes that were greater for modes near the center of the pyramid (Applegate, Ballentine, Gross, Sarver, & Sarver,
2003). This result had been confirmed by other authors using similar methods for stimuli generated computationally (Cheng, Bradley, Ravikumar, & Thibos,
2010) and optically (Chen, Singer, Guirao, Porter, & Williams,
2005; Rocha, Benard, & Legras,
2007; Rouger, Benard, & Legras,
2010; Zhao et al.,
2009). Although it is well understood that the strength of the effect an aberration has on visual acuity varies with the type of aberration, it is important to determine why some aberrations are more detrimental than others.
Figure 1 (right panel) shows each Zernike mode from second to fifth radial order applied to the letter “e”, and it is clear that some modes have a strong effect on the form of the letter, and therefore its legibility, that is different than the rather simple effect of low-pass filtering that is familiar from Gaussian blur transformations. Kwon and Legge (
2013) recently showed that as spatial resolution is reduced by low-pass filtering images of letters, subjects require a higher contrast for letter recognition. They suggested that human performance in such conditions was primarily determined by the information content of the stimuli, which degrades as the blur is increased. How then does this relate to the degradation produced by ocular aberrations? One measure of the performance of an optical system is its optical transfer function (OTF). Contrast changes are represented by the amplitude of the OTF (the modulation transfer function, MTF), and the phase changes are represented by the argument of the OTF (the phase transfer function, PTF). Aberrations that cause spurious resolution, which results from spatial phase changes in the image, have a particularly large impact. Ravikumar, Bradley, and Thibos (
2010) tested the effects of phase errors on the recognition of single letters, letter clusters, and facial expressions under four conditions. These conditions were phase rectification (i.e., PTF set to zero), low-pass filtering (i.e., MTF set to zero for frequencies above the first minimum), low-pass filter plus negative lobes (i.e., including the phase-reversed frequencies), and low-pass filter plus positive lobes (i.e., including the frequencies with the correct phase). They showed that, with sufficient contrast, 180° phase changes can significantly impair visual acuity (size threshold for identification) whereas phase changes of less than 180° had a smaller impact. This implies that even-ordered aberrations, which only cause 0° and 180° phase shifts, are more likely to be detrimental to visual performance than odd-ordered aberrations that cause phase changes between these values. These phase changes are likely to have a particularly detrimental effect when they occur at spatial frequencies that are important for letter identification (see the next paragraph). They additionally showed that the improvement in visual acuity with phase rectification when viewing letter clusters was larger than with single letters. The letter clusters they used were spaced such that crowding effects were likely to occur, and so this improvement is indicative of an effect of crowding associated with phase changes in the stimulus.