We see some similarities between characteristics of suppression obtained with CFS and characteristics of suppression associated with more conventional binocular rivalry stimuli. For one thing, our results reveal that a CFS stimulus composed of only low spatial frequency components is a relatively stronger suppressor than one composed only of high spatial frequencies. The same tendency has been described for conventional binocular rivalry stimuli, where the strength of suppression was gauged by the durations of dominance, suppression, and mixtures of those stimuli (e.g., Hollins & Hudnell,
1980; O'Shea, Sims, & Govan,
1997; Y. Yang, Rose, & Blake,
1992, but see Fahle,
1982). For another thing, we find that CFS induces feature-selective suppression, meaning that losses in visual sensitivity during suppression were strongest when the test probe viewed by one eye and the CFS display viewed by the other eye overlapped in spatial frequency. In addition, an earlier study using the probe technique showed that CFS is also chromatic selective (Hong & Blake,
2009). Conventional binocular rivalry, too, appears to be feature selective, as evidenced by changes in suppression depth for test probes varying in spatial frequency (Stuit, Cass, Paffen, & Alais,
2009), in orientation (Apthorp, Wenderoth, & Alais,
2009; Ling & Blake,
2009; Stuit et al.,
2009; Stuit, Paffen, van der Smagt, & Verstraten,
2011), and in motion direction (Stuit et al.,
2011). We want to clarify, however, what is meant when we use the term “feature-selective suppression.” We do not mean that suppression impacts only a limited range of stimuli (e.g., those initially engaged in rivalry). To the contrary, in nearly all published studies using test probes, suppression tends to adversely impact the visibility of a wide range of probe stimuli presented to an eye during suppression (Blake & Fox,
1974; Fox & Check,
1968; Nguyen, Freeman, & Alais,
2003; Wales & Fox,
1970). Indeed, it is this pattern of results that has led to the characterization of binocular rivalry suppression as “non-selective” (for a review of the literature pointing to that characterization, see Blake,
2001). However, the magnitude of that impairment in visibility
does depend on the specific spatial properties of the stimulus inducing suppression and the stimulus undergoing suppression, and in that sense, we say that suppression is feature selective. In other words, most any new stimulation introduced to an eye whose stimulus is temporarily suppressed will also be adversely impacted (nonselective suppression) but to an extent that depends on the similarity of that new stimulation to the rival targets themselves (feature-selective depth of suppression). Likewise, to reiterate, interocular suppression generally seems strongest when the dominant stimulus shares features with the suppressed stimulus (e.g., Alais & Melcher,
2007; Alais & Parker,
2006; van de Grind, van Hof, van der Smagt, & Verstraten,
2001). It is interesting to note that feature-selective suppression also exists with dichoptic masking (e.g., Baker & Meese,
2007; Harrad & Hess,
1992; Levi, Harwerth, & Smith,
1979), and there is reason to believe that dichoptic masking and binocular rivalry may be mediated by similar interocular suppression mechanisms (e.g., Baker & Graf,
2009a; van Boxtel, van Ee, & Erkelens,
2007).