Up to now, most models of oculomotor distractor effects are based on a competitive integration mechanism of goal-related and stimulus-driven signals (Godijn & Theeuwes,
2002; Trappenberg, Dorris, Munoz, & Klein,
2001). These models share the assumption that competitive integration occurs on a common winner-take-all map with a retinotopic organization which may be found in the intermediate layers of the SC. In the case of neighboring stimuli, when two nearby locations are activated, competitive integration might lead to
cooperation of both signals, i.e. the formation of a single activation peak somewhere between the two stimulus locations resulting in the global effect (Glimcher & Sparks,
1993). In contrast,
competition between signals may arise when two remote locations are activated. Via lateral inhibition, remote distractors might thus slow down the speed at which a threshold for saccade initiation is reached (Godijn & Theeuwes,
2002). To account for distractor effects, competitive integration models assume temporal overlap of the neural activity representing target and distractor location. However, in the present experiments, target, distractor, and saccadic go signal were all separated in time. Yet, a number of studies show sustained neural activity during the retention interval of memory guided saccades at SC neurons representing the target location (Kojima, Matsumura, Togawa, & Hikosaka,
1996; Pare & Wurtz,
2001). This neural memory activity in SC is probably generated by input from higher areas like DLPFC, FEF, and LIP (Johnston & Everling,
2008). It is thus tempting to extend recent competitive integration models by including a memory component.