When similar objects flank a target in peripheral vision, they interfere with the individuation and identification of the target object (Bouma,
1970,
1973; Ehlers,
1936; Korte,
1923; Strasburger, Harvey, & Rentschler,
1991). This interference is commonly called “crowding” (Ehlers,
1936,
1953; Stuart & Burian,
1962; Woodrow,
1938) but has also been called “lateral inhibition” (Townsend,
1971), “lateral interference” (Chastain,
1982; Estes, Allmeyer, & Reder,
1976; Estes & Wolford,
1971; Wolford,
1975), and “lateral masking” (Geiger & Lettvin,
1986; Monti,
1973; Taylor & Brown,
1972; Wolford & Chambers,
1983) (for reviews see: Levi,
2008; Pelli & Tillman,
2008; Strasburger,
2005; Strasburger et al.,
1991; Whitney & Levi,
2011). Crowding persists when cortical magnification is accounted for (Woodworth,
1938), when the observer is given unlimited exposure time (Wilkinson, Wilson, & Ellemberg,
1997), and for a wide range of stimuli (letters: Bouma,
1970; Woodworth,
1938; vernier targets: Westheimer & Hauske,
1975; Landholt Cs: Flom, Weymouth, & Kahneman,
1963; Levi, McGraw, & Klein,
2000). Crowding even persists when the target and flankers are presented to different eyes (Flom et al.,
1963), indicating that it has a binocular cortical mechanism. Crowding-induced deficits in visual perception have been implicated in limiting reading speed (Pelli et al.,
2007), visual search (Vlaskamp & Hooge,
2006), and object perception (Levi,
2008; Pelli & Tillman,
2008). In the midst of these observations, a central question remains: Why does the visual system suffer from crowding?