Color vision helps us to reliably and quickly identify objects within a scene (Bramão, Reis, Petersson, & Faísca,
2011; Tanaka, Weiskopf, & Williams,
2001), both for conditions in which the color of the object is diagnostic for that object (i.e., a yellow banana; Tanaka & Presnell,
1999) as for conditions for which color information is not diagnostic for a particular object, for example a yellow sock (Biederman & Ju,
1988; Bramão, Faísca, Petersson, & Reis,
2010; Gegenfurtner & Rieger,
2000; Uttl, Graf, & Santacruz,
2006; Wurm, Legge, Isenberg, & Luebker,
1993). For perceived surface color to be a useful guide to object identity, it should highly correlate with surface reflectance. This is not easy to achieve because the sensory signal that reaches the eye confounds surface reflectance with the illuminant. The effect of the illumination can be so extreme that the light reaching the eye from a “blue” paper in tungsten light can lead to the same photoreceptor activations as that from a “yellow” paper in sunlight (Jameson,
1985). However, under normal visual circumstances, we are perfectly aware whether we are confronted with either a blue or yellow paper (Granzier, Brenner, & Smeets,
2009a). The ability of the visual system to maintain a stable perception of surface color across changes in illumination (and other viewing conditions) is called color constancy. Without this ability, the color appearance of objects would possibly change from moment to moment, making color information fairly useless for object recognition. The degree to which a human observer is color constant depends on many factors, including both low and high level (cognitive) factors (for a recent overview see Smithson,
2005). Lots of groundbreaking work has been done in the last 30 years with the use of flat, matte surfaces simulated on computer monitors (see for an overview Foster,
2011). The degree of color constancy observed in these experiments was quite variable, but generally far from perfect.