Identification of closely spaced objects is more difficult than for isolated objects, especially in the periphery. This impairment is known as crowding (Bouma,
1970; Flom, Weymouth, & Kahneman,
1963), and the effect increases greatly the further the objects are in the peripheral visual field (Toet & Levi,
1992) and also increases when objects have high similarities (Kooi, Toet, Tripathy, & Levi,
1994). Crowding impairs a wide variety of tasks, ranging from the identification of low-level features like orientation (Andriessen & Bouma,
1976; Westheimer, Shimamura, & McKee,
1976), vernier acuity (Levi, Klein, & Aitsebaomo,
1985; Westheimer & Hauske,
1975), color (van den Berg, Roerdink, & Cornelissen,
2007), stereo acuity (Butler & Westheimer,
1978), and motion (Aghdaee,
2005; Harp, Bressler, & Whitney,
2007; Rajimehr, Vaziri-Pashkam, Afraz, & Esteky,
2004; Whitney & Bressler,
2007), to mid-level recognition of letters (Bouma,
1970; Flom et al.,
1963; Toet & Levi,
1992) and faces (Louie, Bressler, & Whitney,
2007; Martelli, Majaj, & Pelli,
2005), to high-level properties like emotions (Kouider, Berthet, & Faivre,
2011). Crowding has been attributed to the integration of features over an inappropriately wide area (Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, & Morgan,
2001; Toet & Levi,
1992), which is set either preattentively (Pelli, Palomares, & Majaj,
2004) or by the selection region of attention (He, Cavanagh, & Intriligator,
1996; Intriligator & Cavanagh,
2001). When two or more items are within the integration area, their features are combined or exchanged, resulting in degraded performance (He et al.,
1996; Nandy & Tjan,
2007; Pelli et al.,
2004). Previous articles have shown this crowding effect for motion properties, for example, discriminating direction of apparent motion (Rajimehr et al.,
2004), rotational direction (Aghdaee,
2005), and direction of second-order motion (Whitney & Bressler,
2007). It is not clear from these studies at which level the motion properties are degraded by crowding.