Salient items attract not only attention, but also the eyes (Engel,
1977; Godijn & Theeuwes,
2002; Irwin, Colcombe, Kramer, & Hahn,
2000; Ludwig & Gilchrist,
2002; Theeuwes & Godijn,
2001; Theeuwes, Kramer, Hahn, Irwin, & Zelinsky,
1999; van Zoest, Donk, & Theeuwes,
2004; van Zoest & Donk,
2006). Research has shown that, while viewing a natural scene, 60% of eye fixations can be estimated from stimulus saliency, which can be computed from image information (Schütz, Braun, & Gegenfurtner,
2011). In visual searches, saccadic eye movements to a salient item were faster than to a nonsalient item, regardless of the feature dimensions of the salient item (Engel,
1977; Irwin et al.,
2000). Shorter saccade latency was also found for salient, but task-irrelevant, distractors (Ludwig & Gilchrist,
2002; Theeuwes & Godijn,
2001), and potentially biases the saccadic trajectory (Godijn & Theeuwes,
2002; Theeuwes & Godijn,
2001). In addition, a distributional analysis of saccade latency revealed that short-latency saccades usually fall into areas with the highest feature contrast, even when they are not relevant to the task requirement (van Zoest et al.,
2004; van Zoest & Donk,
2006). Later investigation showed that only the initial saccades (the first saccade since the search display onset) and short-latency saccades are signatures for bottom-up defined salience, while second or later saccades or long-latency saccades are determined by a mixed effect of both top-down (e.g., task-relevant, but with smaller feature contrast) and bottom-up (e.g., task-irrelevant, but with larger feature contrast) factors (Donk & van Zoest,
2008; Ludwig & Gilchrist,
2002; Schütz et al.,
2011; Siebold, van Zoest, & Donk,
2011; van Zoest et al.,
2004). In this study, we took the short-latency saccades as markers for bottom-up perceptual salience.