Recent years have seen a surge in microsaccade research (Martinez-Conde et al.,
2004; Martinez-Conde et al.,
2013; Rolfs,
2009). Microsaccades have been linked to the perceptual restoration of faded images (Costela, McCamy, Macknik, Otero-Millan, & Martinez-Conde,
2013; Martinez-Conde, Macknik, Troncoso, & Dyar,
2006; McCamy et al.,
2012; Troncoso, Macknik, & Martinez-Conde,
2008), the visual scanning of small targets (Otero-Millan, Macknik, Langston, & Martinez-Conde,
2013), and the correction of fixation position (Otero-Millan, Macknik, & Martinez-Conde,
2012; Otero-Millan et al.,
2011), among other functions (Martinez-Conde et al.,
2013). Microsaccades can reflect attentional shifts (Engbert & Kliegl,
2003; Hafed & Clark,
2002), modulate the activity of visual neurons (Martinez-Conde, Macknik, & Hubel,
2000;
2002), and show distinctive characteristics in patients affected with ophthalmic and neurological diseases (Chen et al.,
2010; Kapoula et al.,
in press; Martinez-Conde,
2006; Otero-Millan, Schneider, Leigh, Macknik, & Martinez-Conde,
2013; Otero-Millan et al.,
2011; Serra, Liao, Martinez-Conde, Optican, & Leigh,
2008). Thus, improved microsaccade detection has great potential value for the visual and oculomotor research community.