Early psychophysical reports suggest that observers can estimate the direction of gaze of a human subject with very high acuity (Gibson & Pick,
1963; Martin & Rovira,
1981) but that the direction of gaze is influenced by the head orientation (Anstis, Mayhew, & Morely,
1969) in a manner that does not simply reflect changes of the visible part of the eye (Langton, Honeyman, & Tessler,
2004; Todorovic,
2006). More recent accounts have provided different (although not conflicting) explanations for encoding gaze. For example, Ando (
2004) suggests that gaze is estimated based on a local luminance analysis between the eye and the surrounding region, and Calder, Jenkins, Cassel, and Clifford (
2008) propose a multichannel process, whereby the direction of gaze is determined by the relative activity of channels that encode direct and averted gaze. Recently, Gamer and Hecht (
2007) proposed the concept of a cone of direct gaze—the range of directions that were judged as being direct. To measure this, these authors instructed observers to center or decenter the horizontal direction of the eyes in a virtual head and measured the boundaries between direct and averted gaze. Using the decentering technique (a method of limits), they calculated the cone of direct gaze defined as the angular difference between the observer-defined leftward and rightward boundaries as being roughly 8° to 9° in diameter, revealing that observers were quite liberal in their judgments. More recently, a different procedure that bypasses the potential for systematic measurement errors associated with Gamer and Hecht's method of limits (see Hock & Schöner,
2010) was developed. Ewbank, Jennings, and Calder (
2009) and Stoyanova, Ewbank, and Calder (
2010) asked observers to categorize the deviation of gaze in faces as being direct, averted to the left, or averted to the right. With this technique, they were able to reliably measure the influence of facial expression and vocal signals on the cone of direct gaze.