More recently, Xuan et al. (
2007) investigated whether judgments of duration in the range of 600 to approximately 900 ms were influenced by the size of an open square. Applying a Stroop-like interference paradigm, their participants judged the presentation time of open squares while stimulus size was systematically varied. Their data indicated that larger squares were judged temporally longer than smaller ones and, thus, caused Xuan et al. (
2007) to conclude that larger stimuli are perceived to last longer. This claim, however, has been questioned by pointing out that the mere fact that larger stimuli were judged longer than smaller ones does not necessarily imply that the size of a nontemporal stimulus effectively affects perceived duration (Yates et al.,
2012). In fact, it is also conceivable that nontemporal stimulus size simply biases decisions about duration, especially when using a task contingent upon a comparative judgment (cf., Anton-Erxleben, Abrams, & Carrasco,
2010; Nicholls, Lew, Loetscher, & Yates,
2011). In a first experiment employing comparative judgments (i.e., participants judged whether the first or the second of two stimuli was presented longer), Yates et al. (
2012) replicated the effect of stimulus size on duration judgment reported by Xuan et al. (
2007). However, when using equality judgments (i.e., participants judged whether two stimuli were presented for the same duration or for different durations) proposed to measure perceived duration less confounded by decisional bias (cf., Anton-Erxleben et al.,
2010; Nicholls et al.,
2011; Schneider & Komlos,
2008), larger nontemporal stimuli were judged as shorter in duration. Rather than providing converging evidence for the general notion that larger stimuli are perceived to last longer, this unexpected pattern of results emphasized that nontemporal stimulus size may differentially bias decisions about duration as a function of comparative and equality judgments, respectively (Yates et al.,
2012).