Two limitations of our dependent measure bear emphasis. First, the rating scale we used is only a crude index of color appearance, especially when compared to the sensitivity provided by methods like matching or nulling. Subtle changes in hue may often have been insufficient to change the relative ratings for different hue categories. On the other hand, the fact that we could reliably measure hue rotations with this scale indicates that the changes induced by the backgrounds were large and salient. The second limitation is that the ratings measure only the hue of the test color and not its saturation. Consequently our estimates of the underlying sensitivity changes reflect only the selectivity of the change and not its overall magnitude. For example, the results do not reveal whether stronger response changes resulted from adaptation or induction, because they are insensitive to any component of the color change that is nonselective.
With this in mind, the hue shifts we found were consistently more selective following contrast adaptation to the background than from contrast induction to the background. This parallels the results of a number of studies in suggesting that the processes underlying contrast gain control show less stimulus selectivity, and is one source of evidence that the adaptation and gain control are in fact distinct sensitivity adjustments (
Heeger, 1992). However, disentangling the two putative processes is complicated. For example, contrast adaptation effects themselves may include very rapid adjustments (
Muller, Metha, Krauskopf, & Lennie, 1999), and therefore the state of adaptation may have changed substantially during the 500 ms presentation of the test. Moreover, our results do not reveal whether any differences in selectivity are merely a consequence of differences in the magnitude of the sensitivity changes. In any case, the present results suggest that for the conditions we examined, the adaptation and induction influenced color appearance in functionally similar ways. In both cases perceived hue was selectively biased away from the adapting axis, consistent with response changes in multiple color-selective channels, and consistent with the response changes resulting from temporal contrast adaptation. Moreover, the influence of both factors combined in similar ways. As a result, pronounced color biases occurred when observers first adapted to the backgrounds and then judged colors on those backgrounds. As noted above, this would be typical of natural viewing contexts, and suggests that in natural viewing the joint influences of contrast adaptation and contrast induction could strongly modulate color appearance.
The large hue shifts we observed for contrast induction are surprising in light of previous reports of minimal hue shifts (
D’Zmura & Singer, 1999). One possible difference is that the test stimuli we used were highly saturated. However, hue shifts in such stimuli are further surprising because both adaptation and induction tend to have weaker effects on higher-contrast targets (
Georgeson, 1985;
Singer & D’Zmura, 1995;
Webster & Mollon, 1994), and the contrast changes that do persist tend to be nonselective (
Snowden & Hammett, 1992). This is problematic for models that assume that stimulus dimensions like hue are coded by the distribution of channel responses, while contrast is instead encoded by the size of the responses. By such models we should be able to predict the rotations in perceived hue by the changes in perceived contrast or vice versa. Yet the observed rotations imply a selective contrast loss of up to 50% (or more if perceived contrast also decreased along the orthogonal axis), while such large contrast changes were not subjectively evident during the experiment. Moreover, in matching tasks where both components were measured, we have observed significant hue and lightness aftereffects in test stimuli that are little changed in perceived contrast (see
Webster & Malkoc, 2000,
Figure 1). This raises the possibility that contrast, like hue angle, is represented by a distribution of activity across channels
(Webster & Wilson, 2000).
It is interesting to also consider how adaptation or induction might change the perceived hue of test stimuli that were even more saturated than those we used. The test stimuli in our experiments correspond to different ratios of SvsLM and LvsM contrast. This ratio could be biased by changing sensitivity to either cardinal axis. For example, a unique yellow could be shifted toward red or green by adapting to the SvsLM or LvsM axis, respectively. However, a monochromatic yellow falls at a wavelength too long to significantly excite the S cones. Such stimuli might therefore reveal a different pattern of influences (
Webster et al., 2000b).
We were led to these experiments in part by the question of the role that contrast adaptation and contrast induction might play in shaping color vision within different environments. That is, would different color environments hold their inhabitants under different states of adaptation, thus leading them to perceive the same color signals in different ways? These effects could potentially be large. For example,
Figure 14 plots the angles corresponding to the unique hues on the 8 different background axes. The angles were estimated by interpolating between the measured hue angles to find the cone opponent angles that would be rated as pure red, blue, green, or yellow. These stimuli are often measured as the principal directions defining color experience, but as the curves show, they could in theory be strongly influenced by adaptation to a strong bias in the color environment (at least for the moderately saturated stimuli we tested).
However, our attempts to simulate natural viewing provided only partial evidence for color contrast adaptation. When we used natural color distributions to define the spatially random images, the backgrounds induced systematic changes in hue that were consistent with the color variations in the adapting distributions. On the other hand, the present results failed to reveal a contrast adaptation effect when observers adapted to digital images of scenes, even though the color contrast biases in the two cases were comparable. Notably, we also failed to observe evidence for contrast effects when color appearance measurements were made literally within the actual environments while the scenes were being recorded. As part of a different study, two of the authors (MW and SW) judged unique hues in printed palettes in different outdoor settings that included the valley in which the images we used here were taken (
Webster et al., 2002). Even after being immersed in these environments for long periods, their hue settings remained stable. The rich context of actual scenes adds many cues to the nature and origin of color signals, and these cues may mitigate the effects of low-level adjustments of the kind we have considered (
MacLeod, in press;
Mausfeld, 1998). It is also possible that the large average color biases in the scenes (and lack of complete adaptation to this average) reduced the effective contrast variations in the images, or that the test stimuli and backgrounds were somehow mismatched for the natural scenes in ways that prevented their interaction. For example, in the random patterns the test and background elements were chosen to have identical spatial properties, while the circular test differed from the spatial structure of the images of real scenes, which had broad regions of common color, and color variations that were not randomly distributed across the image. Spatial-selectivity of the adaptation or cues to the spatial structure of the scenes might therefore have reduced an influence of the scenes on the color of the test target. Many perceptual judgments of natural scenes can be strongly influenced by contrast adaptation to the patterns in the image (
Webster, in press), and it would be surprising if color were an exception.