Of all the aberrations contributing to the total wavefront aberration of the eye, the terms that showed significant compensation of corneal aberrations by internal optics at relaxed accommodation were horizontal/vertical astigmatism (Z5), lateral coma (Z8), and spherical aberration (Z12). The reduction of the mean magnitude of these aberrations in the complete eye is an important part of producing a retinal image of high optical quality. In the wavefront error due to high-order aberrations (3rd through 6th order), roughly half of the RMS reduction introduced by compensation of corneal lateral coma and corneal spherical aberration (0.114 microns) was still detected in the reduction in total RMS wavefront error (from 0.371 microns corneal RMS to 0.318 microns ocular RMS). That only half is seen is probably attributable to wavefront error introduced by the noncompensated aberration terms, such as vertical coma (Z7), which are included in total RMS.
The compensation of corneal astigmatism by internal astigmatism is well known (Le Grand & El Hage,
1980; Southall,
1937), and so our result of compensation for corneal horizontal/vertical astigmatism (Z5, 41% reduction) by the internal optics is not surprising. Artal et al. (
2001) found the same compensation in term Z5. They also found compensation in triangular astigmatism (or trefoil, Z6), whereas we did not. Our ocular Z6 coefficient is, in fact, larger than the corneal coefficient. This discrepancy may be due to the difference in sample sizes (6 vs. 30 subjects).
The comparison of internal and corneal horizontal/vertical (H/V) astigmatism coefficients for each eye showed a significant negative correlation, indicating that the magnitude of the reduction in the H/V astigmatism of the complete eye is not randomly determined. If overall compensation were due simply to the pairing of a cornea and lens with coefficients inherently opposite in sign, then a mix of over- and under-compensation would be expected. Our data show a consistent match between the magnitudes of the corneal and internal coefficients, which suggests that a process exists to fine-tune H/V astigmatism compensation for the eye, at least in its relaxed accommodative state. Because the matching is so individual-specific, it is imaginable that this fine-tuning process is feedback driven and developmental. It is well documented that the prevalence of refractive astigmatism decreases from infancy to adulthood (Benjamin,
1998; Howland, Atkinson, Braddick, & French,
1978). Perhaps this ontogenetic decrease is due to the progressive development of compensation.
It is interesting to note that while H/V corneal astigmatism (Z5) was significantly compensated, corneal oblique astigmatism, Z3, was not. This may be due to the fact that only 3 of 30 subjects had oblique cylindrical meridians. It is possible that in a larger population of subjects with oblique astigmatism, compensation will be found.
In contrast to H/V astigmatism, spherical aberration does not show individual compensation, but scattered over- and under-compensation regardless of corneal coefficient magnitude (
Figure 7). Our interpretation of this result is that the compensation observed across the sample population is a reflection of the opposite signs of the spherical aberration inherent in the shapes and refractive properties of the cornea and lens. This compensation has been determined over the course of evolution by the feedback-process of natural selection. In one individual lifetime, compensation of spherical aberration is genetically programmed and occurs passively.
Lateral coma shows strong individual compensation. However, the magnitudes of corneal and internal lateral coma are also significantly correlated with the degree of noncoincidence of the Purkinje image and the center of the pupil (which indicates a nonzero angle kappa and/or an eccentricity of the pupil,
Figure 9). The ray-tracing simulations through both the Navarro et al. and the Liou and Brennan schematic eye models (
Figure 10) demonstrate that angle kappa itself may be responsible for some of the compensation of corneal lateral coma by internal coma that we observed in our measurements. Displacement of the pupil, the other cause of noncoincidence of the Purkinje image and the pupil center, does not cause automatic coma compensation in the Navarro schematic eye, but does in the Liou and Brennan eye (
Figure 11). Despite the differences in ray-tracing outcomes for the two models, what is apparent is that shifting the location and alignment of the optical elements of the eye may be a very valid and simple mechanism by which compensation is generated. The individually scaled compensation seen in our lateral coma regression plot (
Figure 6) suggests a process to minimize lateral coma in the eye at relaxed accommodation, which, such as that for astigmatism, could be developmental in nature. Subtle positioning of the optical elements may be a mechanism by which it is achieved.
Relative to increasing ocular total high-order wavefront error, the reduction in RMS error introduced by lateral coma plus spherical aberration compensation (Z8+Z12) seems to be gradually countered by the error introduced by uncompensated aberrations. As seen in
Figure 8a, the RMS magnitude of Z8+Z12 compensation stays within an approximate range for increasing levels of high-order ocular aberration. However, total high-order compensation, the difference in magnitude between ocular and corneal coefficients, decreases with increasing ocular total aberration (
Figure 8b). In more highly aberrated eyes, the uncompensated terms counteract the compensation generated by Z8 and Z12, to the point where total ocular wavefront error is actually greater than corneal wavefront error. The uncompensated RMS error must be primarily generated by the internal optics, because corneal wavefront error does not significantly change as ocular wavefront error increases (
Figure 8c). Thus, that Z8+Z12 compensation occurs does not ensure an eye with low wavefront aberration. However, as lateral coma and spherical aberration are the two high-order terms that are largest in magnitude (
Figure 3), their compensation nonetheless has a role in keeping ocular aberration levels low.
In all of our comparisons, we have said that the internal optics compensate for the cornea, rather than vice versa. This is because as the eye ages, it is primarily the changes in the lens that cause the disappearance of compensation and the augmentation of ocular aberrations (Artal et al.,
2002). The lens continuously grows, and over the course of life, its spherical aberration coefficient reverses sign and increases in magnitude (Glasser & Campbell,
1998). Internal optics aberrations increase three-fold between ages 20 and 70 years, whereas corneal aberrations increase only mildly (Artal et al.,
2002). The decreasing diameter of the pupil with age limits the influence that changes in corneal aberration structure have on image optical quality. Oshika, Klyce, Applegate, and Howland (
1999) found that in a 7-mm pupil, corneal aberrations increased significantly with age, but that for a 3-mm pupil, there was no significant increase.
Our data concerning astigmatism and lateral coma suggest a process to fine-tune the compensation between the cornea and internal optics. In exploring this possibility, we should not make the assumption that the internal optics are determined as a match for the corneal surface. Both elements are capable of changing over time, and we do not know if aberrations in one drive the other or if it is a mutual optimization process.
The time course and mechanism of such an “emmetropization” process are still to be determined. Perhaps a reshuffling of the gradient refractive index of the lens is involved. Previous studies have suggested this as a mechanism for the lens’s maintenance of the same focal length despite changes in surface curvature that occur with age (Glasser & Campbell,
1998; Smith, Atchison, & Pierscionek,
1992). Artal et al. (
2001) suggested that subtle tilting and decentering of the lens to produce lower order aberrations might be a simple way to balance the cornea and internal optics, and we have already mentioned this as a solution specific to lateral coma. Another possibility is that the posterior surface of the cornea has a substantial compensatory role but has not yet been measured (Artal et al.,
2001). Also, the persistence or disappearance of compensation with accommodation has not been addressed here. Future studies on the accommodative and ontogenetic changes in aberration structure should provide further insights.