The stereoscopic stimuli were generated in the form of repetitive autostereograms (
Figure 1) consisting of five horizontal rows of disks with the central line of disks specified as the target (
Minev & Likova, 1999). Vertical distance between the rows was 65 arcmin. The diameter of each disk was 26 arcmin, and their luminance was 52 cd/m
2 against a background of 0.31 cd/m
2. The vertical as well as the horizontal extend of the whole image in all of the experiments did not change (the rows extended across the 29° width of the monitor screen).
The stimuli were typically presented in two frames (
Figure 1), where disparity in the surround disks was changed in order to provide them with alternating stereomotion. The target was either presented in one unchanging disparity plane, or given a disparity alternation that was varied over a wide range in some experiments. The principal frame duration used was 600 ms for each frame (0.83 Hz). However, the phenomena described are not restricted to this frame duration or to the particular sizes and disparities used in the measurements presented here.
The concept of the disparity images generated in space by an autostereogram is depicted in
Figure 2 (
Tyler & Clarke, 1990). The physical autostereogram plane is located at the solid line. To view an autostereogram, the eyes do not converge at the plane of the screen image, but at some other point in space indicated by the intersection of the lines of sight at another location in space. The dotted-line shapes represent the disparity images provided by this particular example of repeat periods. (For this application, the pattern was always uniform in the horizontal direction, with the repeat period varying only in the vertical direction.) These disparity images project into the eyes and up to the visual cortex to generate the corresponding depth percept.
Thus, in the autostereogram presentation technique, the change in surround disparity depicted in
Figure 1 was actually achieved by changing the spacing between each pair of dots from 70 pixels center-to-center to 80 pixels center-to-center (in 1.3′ pixels). The default spacing of the target line was 75 pixels, halfway between these two surround spacings (although other conditions were used in some experiments). At the screen distance of 70 cm, the viewing distance of the target line with uncrossed convergence was 105 cm in terms of its optical geometry. The observer was thus viewing a stereoscopic space behind the computer screen, apparently inside the monitor. Its properties will be specified subsequently in terms of the absolute dot disparities in arcmin.
To evaluate the role of static disparity in the stereomotion induction, we varied the spatiotemporal configuration between the target and the surround disparities (
Figure 3). Absolute surround disparity was varied between 202.5 and 189.5 arcmin, whereas the target row was set at one of five disparities at 6.5 arcmin intervals around the mean surround disparity, giving absolute target disparities of 209, 202.5, 196, 189.5, or 183 arcmin (corresponding to the five locations specified as near, front, mid, back and far in
Figure 3). Thus, surround always jumped back or forth by 13 arcmin of disparity; expressed in terms of optical depth-distances, the two surround distances were 101.6 cm and 108.6 cm, giving a simulated stereomotion in the surround of 7 cm in magnitude. The optical distances of the target locations were 98.4, 101.6, 105, 108.6, and 112.5 cm.
In general, we are using the autostereogram technique as a convenient method of presenting wide-field stereograms without extra hardware devices (for future application in functional magnetic resonance imaging studies). The quality of the depth from autostereograms is equivalent to that from the best dichoptic stereoscopes, so there is good reason to expect that the results should generalize to other methodologies. The advantages of some possible shortcomings are evaluated next.
An important property of these spatially repetitive stimuli is that the monocular motions do not have the right structure to produce coherent monocular-induced motion. The basic manipulation that varies disparity in the autostereogram is a uniform change in the spacing between the dots in a horizontal row without varying the overall width of the display. However, this uniform change generates a variety of monocular local dot motions in the surround. Therefore, if the depth motion were a result of lateral motion induction into the target in each eye, it would be expected to be different at each location along the row. The observers reported, however, that the depth motion seemed quite uniform, having the same amplitude with fixation at any point along the target row. This uniformity is one line of evidence that the target stereomotion was induced by the disparity changes per se, rather than indirectly by summation of the lateral induction effects in the two eyes. A second line of evidence is considered under Experiment 1.