Our results indicate that changes affecting the average statistics across a scene are more likely to be detected. When the isoluminant color changes were in phase with one another (Synchronous Condition) and the average color of the scene varied over time, the changes were more apparent. Previous results suggested that color changes in a scene are more easily detected when accompanied by a change in the average color across a scene. Saiki and Holcombe (
2012) showed subjects an array of dots moving left or right, colored red or green, which evoked a perception of two transparent surfaces moving across one another. At some point in time, all dots swapped color, and when the color and motion of the dots were randomly paired such that the average scene statistics did not change with the color swap, observers were virtually blind to the change. Rich and Gillam (
2000) showed subjects an array of six lines of different colors rotating in depth, which could change color when they moved behind an occluder. They found that observers were far better at noticing when a new color was substituted for an old one than when the lines swapped position. In both these previous examples, the elements were moving at the time of the change, which, as for the Suchow and Alvarez (
2011) study, may have resulted in any transient signals from the color change being attributed to the motion signal. In the case of Rich and Gillam (
2000), the occluder also masked any transient signal that would have resulted from the change. Our results add to these previous findings by demonstrating that even when any transient signals from the color changes are not masked and cannot be attributed to stimulus motion, they are not necessarily effective in attracting attention.
This novel result is consistent with a spatially coarse color change detection mechanism in which changes in the average color across a region are detected, but color changes on a local scale may go unnoticed when the observer's attention is not focused on the area. In this scheme, attention is guided to regions of change by transient signals or changes in the average color on a coarser scale. Such a mechanism would not be engaged by the changes in our Main Condition, in which the average color remains approximately constant, and the changes are not associated with a transient signal.
This interpretation can also account for the finding in
Experiment 1 that in the Main Condition, unlike in other conditions, change detection performance decreased as the number of changing elements increased. We think this finding is most parsimoniously explained by the fact that when the number of changing elements was small the average color of the stimulus changed over time to a greater extent than when the number of changing elements increased. This result is consistent with a spatially coarse color change detection mechanism, which, at the brief presentation time used in our experiment, averages over an area approaching the entire scene. If color changes were detected on a finer spatial scale, increasing the number of changing elements would increase the probability of a detectable change occurring within the integration area of the change detection mechanism, producing increasing, rather than decreasing, change detection performance.
The implication that any isoluminant change detection mechanism operates on a coarse spatial scale cannot be directly linked to the spatial and/or temporal acuity for chromatically defined stimuli, both of which are lower than for luminance-defined stimuli. The rate of alternation of our stimuli (0.33 Hz) is well above the perceptual detection threshold for chromatically alternating stimuli, which is around 15–19 Hz (Wisowaty,
1981; Kelly,
1983; Holcombe & Cavanagh,
2001). Similarly, the spatial scale of our stimuli (each square 1.9° in width) was well within the spatial acuity of mechanisms underlying isoluminant discrimination, which have been estimated for sinusoidally modulating stimuli at around 22 cycles/degree in the fovea and around 12 cycles/degree at an eccentricity of 10° (Anderson, Mullen, & Hess,
1991). The fact that our stimuli are well within these limits means that change detection performance is not limited by the spatial and/or temporal acuity of these detection mechanisms, implying that there is averaging over a much coarser scale in the representation of these stimuli at the level of change-detection mechanisms. The results presented here do not allow a precise estimate of the spatial resolution at which these changes are detected, but variations on the stimulus used here could be used to further constrain the spatial resolution of change detection for isoluminant stimuli.