Examples of the responses to pattern-reversal and pattern-onset stimulation are given for two representative subjects in
Figure 1. These responses were recorded at the Oz-Iz derivation and are spatially arranged as a re-projection of the signals to the visual field locations that evoked them. The original traces are depicted in A and C. The signal-to-noise-ratio (SNR) for each of these traces is depicted in B and C, where symbol size represents SNR magnitude. A signal drop-out (SNR<1.0) is indicated by X, thus representing a spurious scotoma.
Figure 1 demonstrates two typical features of mfVEP recordings that are evident for both pattern-reversal and pattern-onset stimulation: (1) Great variability of the signal strength across the visual field and across subjects. At some locations there is a complete signal drop out (SNR<1.0, indicated by ×), which results in spurious scotomata in the reconstructed visual field map. (2) Variability of the shape of the mfVEPs across the visual field. There is a polarity reversal of the signals along the horizontal meridian (i.e., signals above and below the horizontal meridian tend to have an inverted polarity). It should be noted that this polarity reversal is observed for both stimulus conditions and for both subjects presented. Close inspection of
Figure 1 shows that this high degree of similarity of the trace shapes between the two stimulus conditions can also be observed at visual field locations other than the horizontal meridian. This is underlined by the correlation of the pattern-onset and pattern-reversal responses for these subjects [correlation coefficients (median) in the signal vs. noise window, respectively; J.K.: .69 vs. .04; I.S.: .72 vs. .02] and across the entire 6 subjects tested [correlation coefficients (median, lower, and upper quartile) in the signal vs. noise window, respectively: .62, .45, .75 vs. .07, -.39, .29]. The data, therefore, indicate a similar topographical distribution of pattern-onset and pattern-reversal responses.
Although similar in shape, pattern-onset and pattern-reversal responses exhibit markedly different eccentricity dependences in their response strength. This feature is demonstrated in
Figure 2 and analyzed more formally in
Figure 3. In
Figure 2, a comparison of the signal strength (SNR) for pattern-reversal stimulation and for pattern-onset stimulation is presented for the same subjects that served as examples in
Figure 1. Greater pattern-onset than pattern-reversal responses are indicated by filled symbols, while the inverse relationship is indicated by open symbols. For these illustrations, the most reliable signal (i.e., the signal with the greatest SNR at a specific visual field location) was selected from the corresponding recordings of the six derivations that are obtained after re-referencing the three physical derivations. As is evident from subject J.K., pattern-onset stimulation clearly activates the central visual field (up to 6 or 10°) more strongly than pattern-reversal stimulation, whereas pattern-reversal responses dominate in the periphery. This trend, though less distinct, is also evident from the second example, subject I.S. A quantitative analysis of this feature is given in
Figure 3. Here, we depicted the mean pattern-onset response relative to the pattern-reversal response as a function of eccentricity. A ratio smaller than 1.0 indicates greater pattern-onset responses, while a ratio greater than 1.0 indicates greater pattern-reversal responses. It is evident that pattern-onset responses exceed pattern-reversal responses in the central visual field while they are smaller in the periphery. The results reach significance at the derivations OL and Oz, while the trend is also evident for derivation OR (ANOVA for RMS at OL, Oz, and OR:
p = .0125,
p = .0029, and
p = .22; ANOVA for SNR at OL, Oz, and OR:
p = .0003,
p = .0014, and
p = .11).
It should be noted that pattern-onset mfVEP recordings take twice as long as pattern-reversal mfVEP recordings. The reduction of the recording time for pattern-onset mfVEP by a factor of 2 would yield equal recording times for both stimulus conditions, but is expected to reduce SNRs for pattern-onset mfVEPs by √2. Therefore, SNR-ratios given in
Figure 3 would have to exceed a value of 1/√2 ≈ 0.7 to indicate the dominance of the pattern-onset response for equal recording times of both stimulus conditions.
Finally, we assessed the practical implications of the increased pattern-onset response in the central visual field and compared the number of false positives (i.e., the number of spurious scotomata obtained after pattern-reversal and after pattern-onset stimulation). As for
Figure 2, this analysis is based on the most reliable signals that were obtained at the six derivations after re-referencing. We depicted the number of false positives as a percentage of the visual field locations tested as a function of eccentriticy in
Figure 4. We pooled the two most central eccentricity rings so that 12 visual field locations contribute to each eccentricity bin. Consequently 100% refers to the entire set of 72 visual field locations in a specific eccentricity bin across subjects (12 visual field locations per eccentricity bin X 6 subjects). For pattern-reversal stimulation, we found around 2.5% false positives evenly distributed across eccentricities, while we did not find any false positives for pattern-onset stimulation in the central 6° and only 1.4% false positives within the adjacent annulus from 6 to 10°. It must be noted that beyond this eccentricity false positives reach more than 30% for pattern-onset stimulation and therefore clearly exceed the number of false positives observed after pattern reversal stimulation in the periphery. A combination of both stimuli reduced the number of false positives down to less than 1.5% of the entire 60 visual field locations tested.