Finally, it is important to realize that temporal expectations can be generated in different ways. Temporal expectations can be cued by instructive or predictive stimuli (Cotti, Rohenkohl, Stokes, Nobre, & Coull,
2011; Coull, Davranche, Nazarian, & Vidal,
2013; Coull & Nobre,
1998; Davranche et al.,
2011; Naccache et al.,
2002; Zanto et al.,
2011) as in the present experiment. They can also be induced by rhythmic stimulation (Cravo et al.,
2013; de Graaf et al.,
2013; Jones,
1976,
2010; Jones et al.,
2002; Large & Jones,
1999; Marchant et al.,
2012; Rohenkohl et al.,
2012; Rohenkohl & Nobre,
2011; Snyder & Large,
2005; Zanto, Snyder, & Large,
2006), by manipulating hazard rates (Cravo, Rohenkohl, Wyart, & Nobre,
2011; Cui, Stetson, Montague, & Eagleman,
2009; Janssen & Shadlen,
2005; Vangkilde et al.,
2012), or by the regularity, duration, or sequence of intervals between stimuli in a task (Jepma et al.,
2012; Los & Agter,
2005; Los, Knol, & Boers,
2001; Vallesi, Shallice, & Walsh,
2007). It is not guaranteed that temporal expectations coming from these various sources are all supported by the same neurophysiological mechanisms and have the same consequences for behavior. Some evidence suggests that mechanisms supporting temporal expectations generated by rhythms versus symbolic cues differ (Breska & Deouell,
2014; Coull & Nobre,
2008; Jepma et al.,
2012; Lange,
2012,
2013; Rohenkohl, Coull, & Nobre,
2011; Trivino, Arnedo, Lupianez, Chirivella, & Correa,
2011; Trivino, Correa, Arnedo, & Lupianez,
2010). Future studies are necessary to investigate systematically the ability of these different sources of temporal expectation to interact with other types of expectations.