Some properties that may be important to capture in a complete model of RAP illusory motion are as follows: (1) the direction of motion depends on the order of the colors within the RAP; (2) motion stops after about 6–8 s of steady fixation; (3) when re-fixating a previously fixated point, the time required for motion to stop increases with time away from the point; (4) the eyes need not move to refresh the motion (it suffices to move the pattern, see
Auxiliary Videos 1 and
2); (5) motion is restarted by moderate eye movements, but not by very small ones nor by small amounts of image jitter (
Auxiliary Video 3); (6) the motion can be speeded, stopped, or reversed by preadaptation to specific high-contrast patterns (
Auxiliary Video 4); (7) motion stoppage after monocular viewing does not completely transfer to the other eye (
Auxiliary Figure 3); (8) the motion is more pronounced for binocular than monocular viewing; (9) RAPs evoke a negative motion adaptation aftereffect (Ashida & Kitaoka,
2003); (10) different people see different speeds, and for some RAPs, different directions of motion (Fraser & Wilcox,
1979; Naor-Raz & Sekuler,
2000) and the individual differences are to some extent heritable (Fraser & Wilcox,
1979); (11) the motion falls off rapidly with contrast (Naor-Raz & Sekuler,
2000,
Auxiliary Figure 4); (12) the motion can be enhanced by color for some observers (
Auxiliary Figure 5); (13) the motion is most compelling when the repeated elements of the RAP are configured such that individual local motions, as might be generated by each element, contribute to the same motion within a large image region (
Auxiliary Figure 6); (14) motion in crisp images is most compelling in noncentral vision (Fraser & Wilcox,
1979; Faubert & Herbert,
1999, but see
Auxiliary Figure 7; Naor-Raz & Sekuler,
2000); (15) blur reduces the motion of “Rotating Snakes” in noncentral vision while increasing it in central vision (
Auxiliary Figure 8); and (16) motion is visible in printed RAPs in sunlight.