We think that the inclusion of various additional mechanisms should not change the essential roles of noise and adaptation identified in this study (yet we cannot prove it). We can speculate what these roles mean for the neural correlates of multistable perception. Neural correlates of visual bistable perception have been observed in low-level areas (with fMRI: Tong & Engel,
2001; Wilson, Blake, & Lee,
2001; Haynes, Deichmann, & Rees,
2005; Lee, Blake, & Heeger,
2005,
2007; Wunderlich, Schneider, & Kastner,
2005), high-level visual areas (with monkey electrophysiology: Leopold & Logothetis,
1999; Williams, Elfar, Eskandar, Toth, & Assad,
2003), and in nonvisual parietal and frontal areas (Sterzer & Kleinschmidt,
2007). Among these neural correlates, some may relate to the percept consciously experienced, while others may relate to the mechanism of switching. The distinct roles of adaptation and noise that we have discovered may help in clarifying the apparently conflicting results regarding the brain areas involved in multistable perception. Adaptation is more likely to concern the neural populations that encode each competing percept, and therefore should be observed within the visual cortex. The time course that we observed, for both adaptation of the dominant percept and recovery of the suppressed percept, could be used as a precise signature of the neural correlates. Simply looking for the neural correlates of the perceived interpretations is not decisive, since once an interpretation is selected, it may be both transmitted to higher level areas and fed back to lower visual areas, for example for attention mechanisms (Watanabe et al.,
2011). Looking for the dynamics of the neural correlates of both the suppressed and the dominant percepts would provide much more stringent criteria. Our model revealed the critical role of noise in determining the time of switch. Noise in the model could reflect many different mechanisms, including blinks and non-stimulus-related eye movements (that change the visual input) as well as high-level attention and intention mechanisms. Our proposed role for noise is therefore fully compatible with the involvement of parietal and frontal structures (Sterzer & Kleinschmidt,
2007). However, depending on the content of such noise, prefrontal activity may not be systematically necessary to trigger a switch, if other sources of noise are available.