Having accurate perceptual representations of object size is crucial for interacting with the world around us. However, an object's size is not inherently represented in the size of its projected retinal image. Rather, the perceived size of an object is constructed by integrating multiple sources of information including, but not limited to, retinal image size, physical and perceived distance (Berryhill, Fendrich, & Olson,
2009; Boring,
1940; Emmert,
1881; Ponzo,
1911), an object's geometrical and textural properties (Giora & Gori,
2010; Kundt,
1863; Lotze,
1852; Oppel,
1855; Helmholtz,
1867; Westheimer,
2008), knowledge of an object's typical size (Konkle & Oliva,
2012), and the relative size of different objects in a scene (Coren & Girgus,
1978; Roberts, Harris, & Yates,
2005; Robinson,
1972). The roles these different sources of information play in the construction of perceived size can be revealed through a number of illusions in which the size of an object is misperceived. For example, classical size-contrast and size-assimilation illusions such as the Ebbinghaus illusion (Burton,
2001; Thiéry,
1896) or the Delboeuf illusion (Delboeuf,
1892) demonstrate that the size of a surrounding context can influence the perceived size of a central object (
Figure 1). More recently described illusions, such as the “binding ring illusion” (McCarthy, Kupitz, & Caplovitz,
2013), the “StarTrek illusion” (Qian & Petrov,
2012), the “shrinking building illusion” (Fukuda & Seno,
2011), and the “breathing light illusion” (Anstis, Gori, & Wehrhahn,
2007; Gori, Giora, & Agostini,
2010; Gori & Stubbs,
2006), further demonstrate that the perceived size of an object is influenced by the context in which it is viewed. Together, these illusions have provided insights into our understanding of how we perceive the size of an object. Additionally, understanding such illusions may have practical implications, such as the potential effects of plate size on food consumption (van Ittersum & Wansink,
2012) and the effects of striped clothing on bodily appearance (Ashida, Kuraguchi, & Miyoshi,
2013; Thompson & Mikellidou,
2011). In this paper, we introduce a novel size illusion that highlights the role of visual motion in modulating the contribution of different sources of information in determining the perceived size of an object.